Termout.org logo/LING


Update: February 24, 2023 The new version of Termout.org is now online, so this web site is now obsolete and will soon be dismantled.

Lista de candidatos sometidos a examen:
1) speech (*)
(*) Términos presentes en el nuestro glosario de lingüística

1) Candidate: speech


Is in goldstandard

1
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt591 - : In this story Yunior only produces one inter-sentential Cs: un *chin de respeto (94) to recriminate with a direct speech his brother's behavior when he goes home with one of his girlfriends, and 6 intra-sentential code-switches (5 times as narrator and one as character): *papi chulo (94 ) to describe his brother behavior with women; La Doña (111); Madres de Plaza de Mayo (100) expression to compare these mothers with hers when she asks Rafa for responsibility like this association did in Argentina to free their sons; Pura Mierda (101) in which he plays with the English sounds (r/t) and the Spanish meaning for the sound produced (Puta); Dios Santísimo (96) to refer to what her mother told their four religious Horsefaces friends; “por favor, I spat” (115) to show, in direct speech, his outrage when Pura comes to ask for money.

2
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt539 - : Therefore, when oral production is assessed, fluency is a key element to be considered. Some variables in speech to measure fluency are: speed, hesitation, pauses, and fillers . A similar concept is discussed by ^[54]Schmidt (2008) who referred to fluency as “automatic procedural skills” which means that it is the ability to speak with “little or no effort and attention.”

3
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt495 - : (1a) Te president admires himself ‘el presidente se admira a sí mismo’ (reflexivo). (1b) Te president himself wrote the speech ‘el presidente mismo escribió él discurso’ (no reflexivo: intensificador ).

4
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : For the purpose of my analysis, real speech was not selected, for this usually comprises a number of hesitations, false starts, etc., which would have made the study more difficult to conduct. In lieu of unrehearsed speech, this quasi-spontaneous style was preferred on the following grounds:

5
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : Searle provides a classification of speech acts whose subclassification can surely be furthered on:

6
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : 2.1. Representatives are speech acts that represent some state of affairs in varying degrees of truth with respect to the proposition: state, believe, conclude, deny, report .

7
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : 2.2. Commissives are speech acts that commit – in varying degrees – the speaker to some future course of action: promise, pledge, vow, swear, threat .

8
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : 2.3. Directives are speech acts whose intention is to get the addressee to carry out some action: command, insist, dare, request, challenge, ask, request .

9
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : 2.5. expressives are speech acts that indicate the speaker’s psychological state or mental attitude towards/about a state of affairs: welcome, deplore, greet, thank, congratulate, apologize .

10
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt124 - : However, in the case of indirect directives, a fair number of examples was found where prosody plays a part in changing what seemingly belongs to a different speech act into a directive:

11
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt188 - : The second phenomenon that needs to be investigated within language alternation is interference or transfer. The term interference is defined as, "those instances of deviation from the norm of either language, which occur in the speech of bilinguals as a result of their familiarity with more than one language," (Weinreich, 1953:1 ). In the initial stages of sequential acquisition, when the child has already acquired a first language and is in the process of learning a second, s/he uses the first language system to help construct the second language (Kessler, 1982). This results in transference as normal errors are produced when LI rules are applied to L2.

12
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt334 - : This work will be focused on one secuence in particular: the one represented by the graphemes "hu+vowel". In addition, according to the findings, we postulate and discuss an initial hypothesis about the phonological organization of these segments. In order to accomplish those objectives two instruments are used: The instrument proposed by Cifuentes and Salamanca (2012) - specifically, two out of three tasks contained there- and the out loud reading. All this in order to cover two speech styles: (semi )spontaneous speech and careful speech. Finally, both the findings obtained and the phonemic interpretation hypothesis are compared with the results contained in Calvo (2008).

13
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt125 - : [2] número15 [3]Intonational devices used in the distinction of speech acts [4]Referencias para un análisis del discurso del gobierno militar chileno sobre el movimiento estudiantil universitario: 1973-1980 [5] índice de autores [6]índice de materia [7]búsqueda de artículos [8]Home Page [9]lista alfabética de revistas

14
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt471 - : [RETRACTED ARTICLE] Cognitive problem-solving abilities and speech acts in children: an analysis in vulnerable family contexts

15
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt510 - : ¿Cachái? and their functional equivalents in the Santiago, Chile speech: pragmatic and sociolinguistic analysis of interrogative contact control markers

16
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt286 - : Lambert (1972) shifts away from purely cognitive predictors of language learning success such as aptitude and intelligence, both of which were traditionally regarded as fairly stable determinants, and puts forward a twofold scheme of L2 motivation: integrative vs. instrumental. The first one deals with a desire to 'identify with members of another linguistic-cultural group and be willing to take on very subtle aspects of their behaviour, including their distinctive style of speech and their language', (Lambert, 1972: 290 ), while the second concerns utilitarian purposes associated with L2 learning, with considerably less weight.

17
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt367 - : Chin, S. (1999). The role of the sonority hierarchy in delayed phonological systems. En T. W. Powell (Eds.), Pathologies of speech and language: Contributions of clinical phonetics and linguistics (pp . 109-117). New Orleans, EE.UU.: International Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics Association. [ [39]Links ]

18
paper CL_LiteraturayLingüísticatxt101 - : What is grammar anyhow? Grammar is defined in several different ways. The [25]Oxford American Dictionary, for example, defines grammar as: "the study of words and the rule for their formation and their relationships to each other in sentences; the rules themselves; speech or writing judged as good or bad according to these rules" (1980:282 ). This definition seemed a little narrow for this piece. [26]Patrick Hartwell, author of "Grammar, Grammars, and the Teaching of Grammar", categorizes grammar into five definitions, derived from the likes of W. Nelson Francis and Matha Kolln, summarized as follows:

19
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt48 - : Based on the premise that "human speech has a dual mediational macrofunctions-a primary function, to mediate our social activity, and a secondary function, to mediate our mental activity" (Appel and Lantolf, 1994: 439 ) the researchers designed a study intended to document and analyze the role of private speech (aloud) in two recall tasks. They presented a narrative text and an expository text to a group of fourteen native speakers of English and fourteen speakers of ESL. The students had to prepare a protocol to retell the story to the researchers. During this process, each student would be alone in an office, and his/her speech would be recorded while preparing for the oral report.

20
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt48 - : She found three main characteristics of private speech: 1 ) Vicarious response, which she defines as an answer covertly produced by a learner when the question is addressed to another learner. 2) repetition that is when the learner repeats after the teacher, another student or him/herself; it is covert too and in low voice or whisper and 3) manipulation, and it happens when the learner repeats language forms in order to play with their grammatical, morphological, or semantic structure.

21
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt149 - : the advantages of using this type of corpus over native corpora in terms of quantity and quality of the data base itself. Learner corpus data is usually larger and so provides a wider empirical basis for researchers, whilst having the added strength that "it can be submitted to a wide range of automated tools which make it possible to quantify learner data with linguistic annotations" (Guilquin et al, 2007, p. 324). Regarding error analysis, this research posits that the learner corpus data must be analyzed with software packages that include text-handling tools to facilitate analysis. Unfortunately, there are not many learner corpusdriven studies on speaking skills currently available; nonetheless, Sylviane Grange's research group at Université Catholique de Louvain is working on an investigation of fluency profiles in English learner speech in comparison with native speech: Fluency and disfluency in spoken English (UCL-PhD Theses Under Preparation, 2014 ). It would be interesting to

22
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt58 - : Conceptually, the relationship between thinking and speaking, the view of interaction, and the understanding of learning differ greatly. unlike psycholinguistic approaches that view language and thought as related but completely independent phenomena, socio-cultural approaches see language and thought as highly interwoven processes (Lightbown & Spada, 1999) in which "publicly derived speech completes privately initiated thought" (Lantolf, 2000:7 ). Language mediates thinking and it is through language, either spoken or written, that people gain control over their mental processes. In turn, thought can not be explained without taking language into account (Lantolf and Thome, 2006).

23
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt9 - : The specification of standards and design of tasks revealed important implications for valid and reliable assessments. For instance, in order to reduce unreliability, performance standards need to be clearly stated so that minimal interpretation on the part of the teachers is done and a shared understanding of these standards is developed. Moreover, following the steps to design an assessment task will allow the teachers to elicit from students extended chunks of speech, thus making the assessment easier and more valid. "Requiring extended chunks of speech, with support from the inherent structure of a specific task, will give the student experience in being in charge of the speech situation and responsible for effective communication taking place" (Brown and Yule, 1983:118 ).

24
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt69 - : First, as Scarcella and Rumberger posit, academic writing requires greater grammatical and morphological precision (2000). This academic written discourse is in sharp contrast from communication through speech: the former written discourse is essentially a solitary activity – particularly the composing stage, where ideas are put on paper -reducing the possibility of direct social interaction and feedback with interlocutors . In contrast, speech allows for repetition, backtracking and expansion, all partly conditioned to the interlocutor's reaction to the spoken word, and its level of clarity. These linguistic elements are absent in writing, forcing the writer to keep channels of communication open through conscious selection of vocabulary, sentence structure, and linking devices – all of which contribute to successfully conveying "messages".

25
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt244 - : A large number of studies in ILP have been conducted within the framework of speech acts (^[38]Abed, 2011). According to ^[39]Searle (1969) , “the reason for concentrating on the study of speech acts is simply this: all linguistic communication involves linguistic acts… and speech acts… are the basic or minimal units of linguistic communication” (p . 16). Searle (1969) further argues that speaking, as a rule-governed behavior, has formal features that can be studied independently. However, mere study of those formal features without the study of speech acts is “necessarily incomplete” (p. 17).

26
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt244 - : A review of the speech act theory would be incomplete without touching on the classification systems of speech acts (^[40]Schauer, 2009). One of the most influential classifications of speech acts and the one most constative pragmaticians adhere to (^[41]Robinson, 2006) is ^[42]Searle’s classificatory system. Searle (1975) has identified five classes of speech acts: representatives (e .g., asserting, boasting, claiming, concluding, deducing, describing, insisting, hypothesizing, predicting, reporting, stating, etc.), directives (e.g., begging, commanding, ordering, pleading, requesting, suggesting, etc.), commissives (e.g., offering, pledging, promising, threatening, volunteering, vowing, etc.), expressives (e.g., apologizing, condoling, congratulating, deploring, regretting, thanking, welcoming, etc.), and declarations (e.g., baptizing, christening, firing, sentencing, etc.). This study draws on Searle’s classificatory scheme to investigate Iranian EFL Learners’ realization of the

27
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt244 - : According to ^[56]Ellis (2008) , the study of speech acts in learner language should involve three sets of data: (1 ) samples of the speech act in question produced by L2 learners in the target language, (2) samples of the same speech act as produced by NSs of the target language, and (3) samples of that speech act performed by the learners in their L1. As such, this study included two groups of participants: Iranian EFL learners responding in both English and Persian, and NSs of American English responding in English.

28
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt119 - : ^[43]1Author's data. M stands for Monica, square brackets ([ ]) show pronunciation, the symbol└ shows overlapping speech, successive colons (:: ) denote lengthening of pronunciation, a slash ( \ ) shows falling intonation, capitals show emphasis, and translations appear in double parentheses (( )).

29
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt31 - : Teachers do not have the answers to all these problems. The problems not only concern teachers but society in general. As this is a social issue, a team of professionals such as language teachers, educationalists, translators, psychologists, speech therapists, statisticians should deal with it with the same goal in mind: The solution of problems in educational settings .

30
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt166 - : The power of the epideictic speech as motivation and homonoia strategy: A case of freshmen students at Universidad Distrital

31
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt157 - : Subsequently, Nana begins her assertion by flouting the maxim of Manner through another source of attraction to me as a reader. She uses a figure of speech, a simile, to convey meaning:

32
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt300 - : 1. Spaces of speech: opportunities for the ELT community to engage in a pluralistic and dialogical logic that facilitates the emergence of different knowledges and the construction of other knowledges .

33
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt300 - : Now, to achieve spaces of speech, flattened hierarchy, and teacher agency, I propose a grammar of decoloniality as follows:

34
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt105 - : These six speech acts are generally maintained in the interaction between the teacher and students every time the pattern of asking about content emerges during a linguistic exercise in class. The analysis reveals how the teacher and the students use these six speech acts as components of the functional structure of the interaction in this pattern. The potential influence that these speech acts have on the teacher's and the students' interactional behavior in class seems to depend a great deal on three aspects:

35
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt105 - : To sum up, the interactional pattern of "asking about content" emerges when the students need the teacher's explanation or clarification about the content of a linguistic exercise. The six speech acts in the interactional pattern are generally present in the interaction every time a student asks about content during a linguistic exercise in this class. The potential influence that the six speech acts have on the teacher's and the students' interactional behavior seems to depend on four aspects: on the intention of each speech act within the interactional pattern, on what content the student needs to ask the teacher about, on the level of difficulty at which the student can construct the question, and on what content the teacher uses to answer it .

36
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt105 - : This teacher's speech act emerges and is then maintained in both interactional patterns when the student does not fulfill either of the following two established conventions of the class: 1 ) the student does not add a detailed amount of content to the item in question in a speak-out exercise; and 2) the student does not use the TL for his/her contribution in a speak-out exercise or for his/her question in a linguistic exercise. In both cases, the structure of the interactional patterns is slightly altered:

37
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt79 - : Many Turkish EFL learners struggle with giving complaints and criticisms in the EFL classroom. Language instructors must find way to provide students with the linguistic and pragmatic elements of EFL to be able to appropriately complain as EFL users. The purpose of this study is to investigate the complaint speech used by Turkish EFL learners in two different situations: speaking to a commiserating teacher and speaking to a contradicting teacher . Four kinds of data sources were used to collect data in the classroom: twenty native English speakers' role-plays, twenty-five Turkish native speakers' role-plays, and forty students' role-plays. The subjects' complaint speech act sets were a coding scheme borrowed from a previously conducted study by Murphy and Neu (1996). The baseline and the inter-language data were compared to see to what extent they were similar or different, whether or not the Turkish EFL learners made positive and negative transfer, and if there were any features unique to

38
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt79 - : The analysis of the Turkish data yielded a complaint speech act set which includes the components 'justification', 'candidate solution: request and/or demand', 'complaint', and 'explanation of purpose' . Unlike what Murphy and Neu (1996) found in the native English data set, the analysis of our data revealed that a certain number of speakers produced 'criticism' along with 'complaint', a separate speech act. Also, the type of candidate solution seemed to differ in that Turkish speakers came up with both a request and a demand. One of the sentences uttered by the students to request a solution is:

39
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt79 - : The TNSs produced a complaint speech act set when speaking to a contradicting teacher including the components 'explanation of purpose', 'justification', complaint', 'candidate solution: request and/or demand', and 'criticism' . These components differed from the data produced by those speaking to a commiserating teacher in their use of 'explanation of purpose' and 'candidate solution: request'.

40
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt79 - : The components of the speech act of complaint in the two data sets seem to parallel each other in terms of 'complaint', 'criticism', 'justification', and 'candidate solution: request' . However, in terms of providing 'an explanation of purpose' and 'request as a candidate solution' there is statistical difference in the two sets. This suggests that the TNSs were more likely to explain the reason for their presence in the office of a non-commiserating teacher, and tend to request a solution for an undeserved mark when they speak to a commiserating teacher.

41
paper CO_ColombianAppliedLinguisticsJournaltxt79 - : The speech act set produced by the ENSs included the components' explanation of purpose', 'complaint', 'candidate solution: request', 'justification', and 'criticism' .

42
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt60 - : Searle, J. (1990). Speech Acts: An essay in the Philosophy of language, (1969 ) Actos de habla, Ed. Cátedra, -quinta edición- [ [55]Links ]

43
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt55 - : In this article, two speech comunities from the Universidad de Antioquia are compared: the Faculty of Education and the Faculty of Engineering . A qualitative metodology supported by surveys is used for this purpose. This metodology allows for a detailed analysis of the morfological, pragmatic and semantic features observed in the nominal address formulas (NFA) used by the speakers. In order to analize and compare the information, an emphasis in the lexical families and axiological modalities is made. This method is supported by approaches to the construction of Spanish words proposed by Mervyn Lang, Manuel Casado and Ramón Almena, and also by the studies related to NFA made by Norma Carricaburo. The following phenomena may be found as results of this study: shortening, derivation, diphthongation, appreciative suffixation, and modality/meaning incoherence, among others.

44
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt118 - : Through this strategy, publishers support their argumentation processes in socially accepted sources or experts that validate their positions and arguments against the political scenario of the country with a view to the beginning of the post-conflict. As a discursive strategy legitimizing his speech, the Special Rapporteur uses voices such as: the UN (Organization of the United Nations ), the IACHR (Inter-American Court of Human Rights), expert sources, and consolidated report from several sources of information, different studies, among others. Voices that reinforce the resource of authority, relating the post-conflict process as a responsibility of the State with the preservation of human rights; In order for an appropriate post-conflict process to be reached, it is pertinent to review the actions of State institutions and their responsibility in the restitution of rights violated by their institutions and by other legal and illegal armed actors.

45
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt65 - : The main objective of this paper is to explain the causes behind a metaphorical expression of Venezuelan daily speech: "el papá de los helados" [the father of ice cream] . This research is theoretically supported by the work of Cuenca and Hilferty (1999), Johnson (2006), Díaz (2006), Lakoff and Johnson (2009) and Kövecses (2010). Regarding the methodological design, this is a desk-based, descriptive, interpretative study. The results are the following: (a) the system image of the conceptual metaphor "EL PODER ES ARRIBA" [the power is up] is projected over the system image of "helado" [ice cream] because of its verticality and upwards accumulation. (b) The image of ice cream is used here because sweetness is a metaphor for the sublime. (c) The dominance of the father figure is employed because he is the head of the household, a symbolic authority. (d) In the system image: "el papá de los helados", the biggest ice cream is projected over a man who stands out in a particular aspect. To

46
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt107 - : The Speech of the Twetter: A Linguistic, Sociodiscursive and Sociopragmatic Analysis

47
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt89 - : The objective of this article is to present a revision of the Basic Learning Rights (BLR) and the Basic Standards for Language Competences (BSLC), focusing on the conception of orality proposed in them, specifically for grades sixth to eleventh. The article also makes a comparison between the ideas of some of the most relevant authors in the field of orality the field of orality, whose work has national and international recognition; placing a special focus on the authors who have created bounds between the concepts of orality and performance. As a product of this analysis, new pedagogical strategies will be proposed, based on experiences gathered with the students from the study group El discurso oral en el Quindío: de la Academia a la cotidianidad (DOQAC) [Oral speech in Quindío: from Academia to Everyday life], in order to explore and experiment some of the characteristics of orality that are barely suggested in the BLR and BSCL .

48
paper CO_CuadernosdeLingüísticaHispánicatxt32 - : Space and identity in the speech of Tunja: a gender study

49
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt278 - : Aliaga-García, C. (2011). Measuring perceptual cue weighting after training: a comparison of auditory vs. articulatory training methods. En M. Wrembel, M. Kul, & K. Dziubalska- Kolaczyk (Eds.), Achievements and Perspectives in SLA of Speech: New Sounds 2070 (pp . 12-18). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang [ [162]Links ]

50
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt278 - : Schwarzhaupt, B. M., Alves, U. K., & Fontes, A. B. A. L. (2015). The role of Li knowledge on L2 speech perception: investigating how native speakers and Brazilian learners categorize different VOT patterns in English . Revista de Estudos da Linguagem, 23(2), 3H-334. doi: [228]https://doi.org/10.17851/2237-2083.23.2311-334 [ [229]Links ]

51
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt73 - : With the investiture speech, a president shapes the image of a nation as well as their personal image in a ritual bearing a performative feature. An act of oath, the discourse is important because of its eloquent force and because it constitutes the first statement of a president or Head of State. An inaugural address targets a broad and complex audience, even an international community. This article analyzes the features of inauguration speech from the analysis of the text of the take of possession for the consecutive term of two Latin American presidents: Álvaro Uribe Vélez, sworn on the 7th of August of 2006, and Hugo Chávez, who resumed the presidency of his country on the 10th of January of 2007 . The article also studies, in each discourse, the use of the concept of a nation and the semantic boundaries of this word to determine the pragmatic outcomes as the result of its use. The method employed in the study was Critical Discourse Analysis, one with a critical intent, as defined by

52
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt253 - : INTOLERANT SPEECH ON THE INTERNET: THE BRAZILIAN CASE OF SEXUAL RANKING AT THE UNIVERSITY OF SAO PAULO

53
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt170 - : to be made, and at the same time process the literal meaning as it adds the politeness to the speech act, as in the following exchange:

54
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt170 - : Finally, it must be mentioned that we encountered in our analysis what we call "extended speech acts": in some cases, two or more instances of realizations for a single speech act were identified in one particular interaction . This occurs because the speakers sometimes rephrase their speech acts with one or two more direct or less direct utterances than the first one. Thus, we consider the subsequent realizations of the act to be an extension of the speech act in the interaction rather than a new one. This can be seen in the following conversational exchange, where the second realization is a way to indirectly paraphrase the first:

55
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt170 - : As regards the case in which the translated politeness was higher than in the ST, in 6 of the interactions some evidence was found of what seems to be an attempt to adapt the politeness of the speech act to the target culture, as in the following exchange where the translated speech act includes one more instance of redress —the lexical downgrader please, making the TT exchange "more polite" than the one in the ST:

56
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt20 - : "Smart talk" is an overall characteristic of African American womens'speech. Black women use language in an assertive, bold, outspoken manner. In a conversation among three women friends, one Woman remarked, "Im glad I dont have a man around ‘cause I can do whatever the hell I want to do." Terry McMillan, in her 1992 novel Waiting to Exhale, creates authenticity in her women characters by the use of "smart talk." McMillans main character, Savannah, punctuates her sentences with this style of speech from the novels beginning: "Sheila, my baby sister, insisted on giving me his [Lionels] phone number because he lives here in Denver and her simpleass husband played basketball with him eleven years ago at the University of Washington ." This feature of Black womens'discourse departs from the so-called "code of feminine politeness" characteristic of European American women. Instead of Marilyn Fryes depiction of women who "live in cages", that is, women who know their "place", African American

57
paper CO_Lenguajetxt7 - : The content of the message is called now a "locution" (direct or indirect), but it is not analyzed as a participant in the verbal process because it is found outside the verbal clause. When the message is expressed through reported speech, the logico-semantic relation between a primary and a secondary member of a clause nexus is called "projection" in functional grammar (Halliday, 2004: 376 ). This relationship between the two members is like the one "between a picture (projected clause) and its frame (projecting clause): together they make up a single complex unit, but neither is actually part of the other" (Thompson, 2004: 103).

58
paper CO_Lenguajetxt89 - : Humor and Speech: A Proposal for the Analysis of the Protest on YouTube

59
paper CO_Lenguajetxt112 - : Van der Houwen encuentra, entre otros aspectos, que los hablantes usan más el DD que el DI: “indirect speech is the marked strategy (15%) and direct speech the unmarked strategy (total: 85% )” (^[60]Van der Houwen, 1998, p. 129). De igual manera, encontró que se elide más el sujeto en el DD, pues su resultado “shows the expected correlation between the presence of the subject and indirect speech: 30% (vs. 15% of absent subjects)” (^[61]Van der Houwen, 1998, p. 129). A partir de estos resultados, ^[62]Van der Houwen (1998) concluye que “indirect and direct speech, indeed, each have a distinctive functional communicative effect. Their distribution is not random, but depends on the message the speaker wants to convey” (p. 132).

60
paper CO_Lenguajetxt164 - : * The University of Iowa: Sounds of Speech: [85]http://soundsofspeech .uiowa.edu/index.html#english.

61
paper CO_Lenguajetxt33 - : The discussion focuses on both the theoretical delimitation of speech variety and the mechanisms of meaning construction, such as: neologisms, synonymy, polysemy and semantic transformation . The results highlight the function and meaning of this argot and its signic productivity, as well.

62
paper CO_Lenguajetxt170 - : Metalinguistic speech and contextualization: Interlinguistic descriptions of French teachers in Mexico

63
paper CO_Íkalatxt127 - : Several studies have noted that RPs are a more valid measure of authentic language use compared to DCTs (Eisenstein & Bodman, 1993; Félix- Brasdefer, 2003; Tran, 2006). As Kasper (2000) points out, in comparison to questionnaire data, RP data capture many of the discourse features found in natural speech: intonation, pauses, turn-taking, overlap and laughter, among others . Role-play interactions also elicit more negotiation, repetition and supportive moves in comparison to written methods (Beebe & Cummings, 1996; Félix-Brasdefer, 2003; Margalef-Boada, 1993).

64
paper CO_Íkalatxt127 - : 6. Beebe, L. M. and Cummings M. C. (1996). Natural speech act data versus written questionnaire data: How data collection method affects speech act performance . In S. M. Gass and J. Neu (Eds.), Speech acts across cultures: Challenges to communication in a second language, (pp. 65- 86). New York, NJ: Mouton de Gruyter. [ [53]Links ]

65
paper CO_Íkalatxt202 - : Dewaele, J.-M. (2004b). Perceived Language Dominance and Language Preference for Emotional Speech: The Implications for Attrition Research . In M. Schmid, B. Köpke , M. Keijzer, and L. Weilemar , (Eds.). First Language Attrition: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Methodological Issues (pp. 81-104). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [ [114]Links ]

66
paper CO_Íkalatxt11 - : Polite behaviour is subject to the features of the interaction which are socioculturally marked by the speech community beyond what is regarded as political behaviour. Thus, Brown and Levinson's strategies of positive and negative politeness are interpreted as socio-culturally determined politic behaviour. Likewise, it will have to pay attention to whether examples of linguistic politeness such as terms of address, honorifics, ritualised expressions and speech events, and indirect speech acts are polite forms or whether they are used normally as socio-culturally constrained forms of politic behaviour (Watts, 1992:51 ). Therefore, politic behaviour is just a socially appropriated behaviour and terms of address are realisations of politic behaviour.

67
paper CO_Íkalatxt12 - : As can be seen in [43]Table 10, none of the participants' background characteristics and few of the language contact variables measured resulted in statistically significant associations with the rated performance gains in the request and apology vignettes or composite scores. The following variables were not found to be related to speech act gains: gender, year in university, region of study abroad site, country of study abroad, amount of previous formal study of Spanish, amount of previous residence outside NorthAmerica, living arrangements, conversation partner, internship, type of classes taken, and amount of time spent outside of class speaking Spanish with native or non-native friends .

68
paper CO_Íkalatxt12 - : 31. Kim, I. (2000). Relationship of onset age of ESL acquisition and extent of informal input to appropriateness and nativeness in performing four speech acts in English: A study of native Korean adult speakers of ESL . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University. [ [77]Links ]

69
paper CO_Íkalatxt31 - : Language policies are interventions that take place to modify the use of a given language in a community. They are the expression of beliefs and ideologies that go beyond the academic sphere because they respond to economic and political agendas that may be open or hidden (Shohamy, 2006; Spolsky, 2004). Designed by politicians, but implemented by teachers, language policies are often top-down approaches. Spolsky (2004, p. 5) notes that policies are easily recognized when they ''exist in the form of clearcut labeled statements in official documents''. The author identifies three components in the language policy of a speech community: its language practices, its language beliefs or ideology and the specific efforts to modify that practice . Shohamy (2006, p.48) says that it is ''through language policies that decisions are made with regard to the preferred languages that should be used, where, when and by whom''. Tollefson (1991) sees language policies as an instrument of inequality as they

70
paper CO_Íkalatxt216 - : Strange, W., y Shafer, V. L. (2008). Speech perception in second language learners: The re-education of selective perception . En J. G. Hansen Edwards y M. L. Zampini (Eds.), Phonology and Second Language Acquisition (Vol. 36, pp. 153-191). Ámsterdam, Filadelfia: John Benjamins Publishing. [ [159]Links ]

71
paper MX_ElAnuariodeLetrastxt37 - : Among the different devices deployed by those who make political speeches are those of an architectural kind, which form the skeleton of the dissertation. These are certainly the least studied in the analysis of discourses of this kind, although its proper use is very often pivotal to better structured, more cohesive and more intelligible presentations. In this paper we concentrate on the speech given by Pedro Sánchez on 31 May 2018 on the occasion of a motion of censure presented by his political party, the Socialist Group, against the then president of the Government Mariano Rajoy. We segment this speech into three sequences: beginning, development and closing . The main focus of this article is thus on the structure of these sequences as well as those devices giving cohesion and coherence to the discourse as a whole, as signals of thematic shift, while also taking on board non-architectural strategies (e.g. highlighting, masking), which are closely connected with architectural ones.

72
paper MX_ElAnuariodeLetrastxt6 - : The Reported Speech Use in the PRESEEA Corpus of Seville: the High Sociolect

73
paper MX_ElAnuariodeLetrastxt45 - : The objective of this paper is to describe the prosodic features of the final intonation contour of minor intonational phrases (ip) and the tonemes of major intonational phrases (IP) in Mexico City’s Spanish variety. The speech data was taken from a spontaneous speech corpus made from speakers from two social networks: neighborhood and labor . Final intonation contours of ip show a predominantly rising movement. These contours are generally produced with greater length in the last syllable of the ip, which represents the most significant difference between both networks in the case of oxitone endings. On the other hand, tonemes are predominantly descendant, although the circumflex accent has an important number of cases within the data set. Tonemes produced by the neighborhood network are produced with larger length than those from the labor network.

74
paper PE_Lexistxt9 - : El tercer capítulo se dedica al análisis del sintagma nominal, que reclama de partida su carácter nuclear dentro de la obra: "Together with the restructured verb phrase, the Afro-Yungueño noun phrase represents one of the most significant departures from patrimonial Spanish syntax, and provides some of the most compelling evidence linking the traditional Afro-Bolivian dialect with other Afro-Hispanic speech communities, and with Afro-Iberian creoles languages" (81 ) (tal vez esto último justifica el que, a partir de este momento, provisionalmente, el autor asume a veces conceptos tomados de la Criollística, como "post-creole continuum", p. 81, para referirse a los cambios entre la variedad más vernácula y las formas más próximas al español general de las tierras altas de Bolivia, problema que abordará definitivamente en la última sección del libro: 177-197).

75
paper PE_Lexistxt52 - : La adquisición de las oposiciones sigue más o menos un orden universal pero siempre aproximándose al sistema fonológico de la lengua que se está aprendiendo. En Preliminaries to Speech Analysis, Jakobson, Fant y Halle sostienen que los sistemas fonológicos o "patrones fonémicos" se organizan de acuerdo con una "escala dicotómica [que] la lengua superpone a la materia sonora" (1952: 12 ). Esta escala dicotómica determina las unidades fonológicas que usa cada lengua mediante rasgos distintivos binarios que establecen distinciones cada vez más específicas. Siguiendo esta misma línea de pensamiento, en Fundamentals of Language, Jakobson y Halle presentan de manera sintética la forma en que se organizan jerárquicamente los fonemas consonánticos del francés, los cuales "requieren solo cinco decisiones binarias: nasal/oral, y si es oral, entonces continuo/discontinuo y tenso/laxo; compacto/difuso, y si es difuso, entonces grave/agudo. Cada consonante del francés contiene desde dos (compacto nasal)

76
paper PE_Lexistxt109 - : ^[60]Foulkes, Docherty y Watt (2005) estudiaron los rasgos segmentales en el CDS (Child Directed Speech: lenguaje dirigido a niños ) en un corpus de treinta y nueve madres inglesas (Tyneside). La atención se centró en las variantes fonéticas utilizadas para el sonido [t]. La primera sería la estándar [t] como oclusiva áfona, mientras que la segunda correspondería a una variante glotal [h]. Ambas variantes muestran un claro patrón sociolingüístico en la comunidad adulta. Entre sus principales hallazgos, evidenciaron que el uso de variantes en el CDS difiere marcadamente del uso en el habla entre adultos. Específicamente en el habla a los niños, el uso de [t] aumenta a un promedio del 59 %, en contraste con el promedio del 10 % utilizado en el modo interadulto, mientras que se observó un uso de glotales del 36 % en el CDS frente al 90 % de uso de esta variable entre los adultos.

77
paper UY_ALFALtxt68 - : [114]Gardiner, Alan. 1932. The theory of speech and language, Oxford: Clarendon .

78
paper UY_ALFALtxt204 - : Fey, Marc, Hugh Catts, Kerry Proctor-Williams, Bruce Tomblin y Xuyang Zhang. 2004. Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment, Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47: 1301-1318 (en línea) Disponible en [150]https://doi .org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/098) [ [151]Links ]

79
paper UY_ALFALtxt204 - : Ukrainetz, Teresa y Ronald Gillam. 2009. The expressive elaboration of imaginative narratives by children with Specific Language impairment, Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 52: 883-898 . (en línea) Disponible en [189]https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0133) [ [190]Links ]

80
paper UY_ALFALtxt193 - : Children’s speech out of time and place:On conflictual relationships in the mother tongue

81
paper UY_ALFALtxt244 - : Baumann, Stefan e Jürgen Trouvain. 2001. On the prosody of German telephone numbers, em Paul Dalsgaard, Borge Lindberg e Henrik Benner (eds.), Proceedings of the 2th INTERSPEECH and 7th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (EUROSPEECH 2001), Aalborg, Darst: 557-560 . [ [170]Links ]

82
paper UY_ALFALtxt113 - : Alves, Ubiratã Kickhöfel y Márcia Cristina Zimmer. 2012. The dynamics of perception and production of VOT Patterns in English by Brazilian Learners, em E. Mello, M. Pettorino y T. Raso (eds.), Proceedings of the VIIth GSCP international conference: speech and corpora, Firenze, Firenze University Press: 223-227 .

83
paper UY_ALFALtxt113 - : [164]Yavaș[165], Mehmet. 2008. Factors influencing the VOT of English long lag stops and interlanguage phonology, en A. S. Rauber, M. A. Watkins y B. O Baptista (eds.), New Sounds 2007: Proceedings of the fifth international symposium on the acquisition of second language speech, Florianópolis, UFSC: 492-498 .

84
paper UY_ALFALtxt234 - : It is unquestionable that children's speech can be highly unpredictable to an extent that it may touch “a language consolidated limit” (^[29]Figueira 2015: 174 ). In such cases, a major cut-off effect ends up separating a child's speech from the language set conceived of as "normal speech”. In fact, symptomatic speech troubles the communicative dynamics in interaction situations and deeply affects what is assumed as pragmatic (in)flexibility. In order to reflect on that theme (pragmatic flexibility) when symptomatic speech is at stake, the present work discusses some child-adult dialogues in clinical situations. The analysis is based on European Structuralism and, on the speaker, Psychoanalysis. We conclude that identifying pragmatic flexibility in children with symptomatic speech does not help the recognition of singularity of the infantile speech manifestations.

85
paper UY_ALFALtxt112 - : In the field of speech-language pathology there´re researches that explore a range of issues related to looking after people with Cerebral Palsy. They´re oriented by social-cognitive aspects. The absence of speech, very common in severe cases of the disease, may have a role in this case: researchers and practitioners conclude that if there´s no talk, the person is out of language . It´s assumed the person, however, perceptual and cognitive capacities, but not languages. Under such kind of reasoning, language and its effects on the subject are ignored. This traditional framework exception is a discussion that another theory is consistently about language and about subject (Vasconcellos 1999 and 2010; Dudas 2009). Summarizing, are very rare the researches that distance themselves from the speech and organization epistemic subject. This article takes the direction of the exception mentioned to reflect on the institutionalization of people with Cerebral Paralyses and subjective and clinical

86
paper UY_ALFALtxt205 - : In order to understand how children learn to recognize and use humor in their speech, we have chosen to study their linguistic production in two different languages and cultures: a French-speaking monolingual child and a Brazilian Portuguese-speaking child, video-recorded once a month in a longitudinal study, whose data were analyzed for this article between 24 and 42 months . The results of the multimodal linguistic coding of our data enabled us to describe the paths the two children followed to get from first instances of shared amusement initiated by the adult to their own verbal production of successful humor in dialogue. The different parameters required for humor are set from the age of 2;4 years old. Our study demonstrates that the production of children’s humor is closely linked to the family input and to children’s multimodal linguistic and meta-cognitive development. We did not observe important differences between the two children at the macro-cultural level, but there were some

87
paper UY_ALFALtxt228 - : This article deals with two conceptually correlated phenomena, Reported Speech and Fictive Interaction (^[36]Pascual 2014), based on the theory of fictivity (^[37]Talmy 2000) about discrepant representations of the same object: discourse . It is postulated that RS and FI trigger the Conversation Frame to structure the discursive construction. The former is taken as factive or genuine and the latter as fictive or not genuine. In the search for empirical evidence in C-ORAL-BRASIL I (^[38]Raso e Mello 2012), informal spontaneous speech corpus of Brazilian Portuguese, it was verified that instances of Reported Speech represent factual problems, while of Fictive Interaction, pro-factual solutions, according to the discursive pattern "Problem-Solution" (^[39]Hoey 2001). In addition, it has been observed that such instances, even though they carry unique illocutionary acts, are grouped into attentional frames (^[40]Langacker 2008) that transcend the uniqueness of the informational components,

88
paper UY_ALFALtxt250 - : Based on the Textual-Interactive Grammar (TIG), we investigate the construction “(eu só) sei que” in its function of Discourse Marker (DM) acting in the textual organization. Based on data collected from speech samples of Brazilian Portuguese variety, we identify five textual functions of DM: topic closing ; intratopic sequencing; topic sequencing; topic opening and basically sequencer + secondarily modal. We conclude that these five functions can compound to a path of linguistic change, via abstraction of uses, having as terminal focal point the DM functions.

89
paper UY_ALFALtxt212 - : Félix-Brasdefer, J. César. 2010. Data collection methods in speech act performance: DCTs, role plays, and verbal reports, em E . Usó-Juan e A. Martinéz-Flor (eds.), Speech act performance: theoretical, empirical, and methodological issues, Amsterdam, John Benjamins Publishing: 41-56. [ [218]Links ]

90
paper UY_ALFALtxt194 - : This paper aims at discussing some theoretical-methodological aspects related to the way children’s speech data are approached in language diagnostic procedures by speech-language pathologists. It is assumed the deep discontinuity between living speech and speech registers: there is a marked difference between these two instances . The questions raised in this article are: i) what, from living speech, remains accessible to the researcher?, and ii) what remains from living speech in the clinician’s listening when he deals with recorded speech materials?

91
paper UY_ALFALtxt207 - : Casillas, Marisa, Andrei Amatuni, Amanda Seidl, Melanie Soderstrom, Anne Warlaumont, y Elika Bergelson. 2017. What do Babies hear? Analyses of child-and adult-directed speech, Interspeech, 2017: 2093-2097 . [ [126]Links ]

92
paper UY_ALFALtxt207 - : Soderstrom, Melanie y Kelsey Wittebolle. 2013. When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments, PLoS ONE, 8,11: e80646 . en línea. Disponible en [167]https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080646 [ [168]Links ]

93
paper VE_BoletindeLinguisticatxt96 - : 19.Dellwo, Volker. 2006. Rhythm and speech rate: a variation coefficient for delta C . En Pawe Karnowski e Imre Szigeti (eds.), Sprache und Sprachverarbeitung: Akten des 38. Linguistischen Kolloquiums in Piliscsaba 2003/ Language and language-processing: Proceedings of the 38th Linguistics Colloquium, Piliscsaba 2003, Linguistik International 15. 231-241. Frankfurt am Main, Alemania : Peter Lang Publishing Group.

94
paper VE_BoletindeLinguisticatxt96 - : 38.O’Rourke, Erin. 2008 b. Speech rhythm variation in dialects of Spanish: Applying the Pairwise Variability Index and Variation Coefficients to Peruvian Spanish . En Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008: Fourth Conference on Speech Prosody. 431-434. Campinas, Brasil.

95
paper VE_Núcleotxt38 - : Dual Speech Acts: A Reflection on the Pragmatics of Spanish regarding Current Theories and Methods of Analysis

96
paper VE_Núcleotxt38 - : 1 “Some, if not all speech acts in Cuban Spanish (such as requests, orders, and compliments…), which were universally classified by Brown and Levinson as FTAs to the Hearer’s negative face, then have, under my hypothesis, a lesser potential to be threatening or perceived as such in this language-specific cultural setting” (Ruzicková, 1998: 305 ).

97
paper corpusLogostxt47 - : Boxer, D. (1995). The ethnographic interview as a tool for speech act research: The case of complaints . [107]http://cpd1.ufmt.br/meel/arquivos/artigos/184.pdf (consulted in November 2016). [ [108]Links ]

98
paper corpusLogostxt137 - : The referred speech is usually presented by declarative verbs. However, there are other semantic classes specialized in speech reproduction procedures, such as request verbs. From the analysis of the Archivo de Textos Hispánicos de la Universidad de Santiago, made up of texts of oral, theatrical, journalistic and narrative language, we try to establish the frequency of use of the verbs implorar, suplicar, rogar, solicitar, pedir, reclamar and exigir as introducers of referred speech, as well as their greater or lesser productivity according to the type of procedure: indirect style, direct style, pseudo-direct style and hybrid style . In addition, the characteristics of each structure are explored with the proposal of knowing the configuration mode of the sequences that introduces each verb (its members, the position of each element, etc.) and that establish certain differences between them within each type of referred speech.

99
paper corpusRLAtxt235 - : This article analyzes the pragmatic competence in the Spanish interlanguage through the study of a speech act: the requesting . For that, 40 text productions from the Corpus de Aprendices de Español (CAES) (Instituto Cervantes and Universidade de Santiago de Compostela), made by Spanish as a Foreign Language (SFLE) A2 students, were examined quantitatively and qualitatively. The data was compared with a sample from 22 Spanish native speakers with the same social profile: male and female with high level studies and ages from 18 to 40 years old. Results show that Spanish learners know the characteristics of this speech act in the target language, but they do not recognise some linguistic and discourse patterns of its expression. For this reason, we propose some improvements for the teaching of the pragmatic component in the SFL classroom.

100
paper corpusRLAtxt54 - : Law, James; Boyle, James; Harris, Francis; Harkness, Avril y Nye, Chad. 2000. "Prevalence and Natural History of Primary Speech and Language Delay: Finding from a systematic review of the literature", en International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders . 35 (2), pp. 165-188. [ [66]Links ]

101
paper corpusRLAtxt217 - : successfully overcome any possible face-threating situation. As a result, an important pedagogical implication of the study is that "teachers must pay more attention to pragmatic knowledge and try to inform students about the appropriate […] behaviours used by […] native speakers. It is necessary for students to be aware of [native] socio-cultural norms of speech act realizations" (^[126]Zarepour and Saidloo, 2016: 584 ). Sociopragmatic competence may require the implementation of more intensive bilingual programs, where the foreign language is a means of communication not only with the teacher, but also among peers (^[127]Lindholm-Leary, 2016).

102
paper corpusRLAtxt177 - : Félix-Brasdefer, J. César. 2010. Data collection methods in speech act perfor mance: DCTs, role plays, and verbal reports . In Alicia Martínez-Flor and Es ther Usó-Juan (eds.), Speech act performance: Theoretical, empirical, and meth odological issues, 41-56. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [ [138]Links ]

103
paper corpusRLAtxt220 - : Eadie, Patricia; Morgan, Angela; Ukoumunne, Obioha C.; Ttofari, Eecen Kyriaki; Wake, Melissa y Reilly, Sheena. (2015). Speech sound disorder at 4 years: Prevalence, comorbidities, and predictors in a community cohort of children . Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(6), 578-584. doi: 10.1111/ dmcn.12635. [ [108]Links ]

104
paper corpusRLAtxt86 - : En el área de adquisición de segundas lenguas en Lingüística Aplicada, las investigadoras brasileñas Kyria Finardi y Mailce Borges Mota, de la Universidad Federal del Espíritu Santo y de la Universidad Federal de Santa Catarina, presentan el artículo "The acquisition of a syntactic structure in L2 speech: The role of working memory capacity" . Sobre la base de la Teoría del Procesamiento de Información, se investiga la relación entre la capacidad de memoria operacional y la adquisición de una estructura sintáctica en el habla del L2. Los resultados evidencian el aporte lingüístico del procesamiento en L2 para complementar y explicar la adquisición de algunas estructuras sintácticas. Además se analiza la discusión entre el procesamiento de la forma versus el significado, la adquisición de estructuras basadas en reglas, las variaciones lingüísticas en L1 y L2 y las limitaciones de la producción oral en L2.

105
paper corpusRLAtxt4 - : Curiously, postnuclear, labial obstruent /p/ is realized as [k] quite commonly according to Canfield (1981). This phonetic variant is frequent in the casual speech style of Venezuela, Colombia, Central America (Canfield, 1981: 11 ) and is also found in the Caribbean islands and other areas of the Spanish speaking world (Zamora Munné & Guitart, 1982; Betancourt Arango & García Zapata, 1998; Fraca de Barrera & Obregón Muñoz, 1985; Quilis, 1993; Salvador, 1987). Examples can be seen in (1).

106
paper corpusRLAtxt110 - : Hence, one of the first problems that language learners have to face is the continuous nature of the speech stream. Speech lacks consistent physical markers of the location of word boundaries: words in fluent speech are not separated by pauses, or signaled by any other consistent feature occurring only at word onsets or endings (Aslin, Woodward, LaMendola & Bever, 1996 ; Cole & Jakimik, 1980). The fact that there are no consistent boundaries raises a difficult problem for children, who must anyway determine which sound strings are words and which are not.

107
paper corpusRLAtxt91 - : The acquisition of a syntactic structure in L2 speech: the role of working memory capacity

108
paper corpusRLAtxt176 - : COMO (QUE) AND ITS DISCURSIVE FUNCTIONS IN THE SANTIAGO, CHILE SPEECH: PRAGMATIC AND SOCIOLINGUISTIC ANALYSIS

109
paper corpusRLAtxt116 - : Chin, Steven. (1996). The role of the sonority hierarchy in delayed phonological systems. En T. W. Powell (ed.), Pathologies of speech and language: Contributions of clinical phonetics and linguistics (pp . 109-117). New Orleans, LA: International Clinical Phonetics and Linguistics Association. [ [55]Links ]

110
paper corpusRLAtxt141 - : DIATOPIC VARIATION IN THE SPEECH OF CHILEAN PROFESSIONALS: SUBJECTIVE ASSESSMENT

111
paper corpusSignostxt426 - : “a rhetor must modulate his speech for the old, the young, and the middle-aged, groups whose habits of mind create an exigency that must be accounted for in the invention process” (Mountford, 2001: 41 ).

112
paper corpusSignostxt426 - : In this respect, in spite of their distinct names in different disciplines, and considering the further definitions by Werth and Zarefsky, one can draw a parallel line between rhetorical space in Rhetorical Studies and discourse in Linguistics, the latter one defined as “a combination of text and its relevant context” (Werth, 1999: 47), where the context refers to “the situational context surrounding the speech event itself” (Werth, 1999: 83 ), and the text to both oral and written discourse. In the first case, it takes the form of transcription of speeches, which are normally used for this type of studies.

113
paper corpusSignostxt426 - : that “based on the cohesive ties among the various utterances of the discourse, there are several potential referents of the indexicals” and “hearers may choose to include themselves as members of the class of referents” (Zupnik, 1994: 340). Thus, it may facilitate the achievement “of the main goal of political speech: to persuade listeners of the speaker’s viewpoint” (Zupnik, 1994: 340 ).

114
paper corpusSignostxt426 - : It is interesting to stress out the ambiguous and inclusive use of the pronoun nosotros (we) by Bachelet in her 2013 victory speech, which is “a piece of political communication intended for a wide distribution” (Mulderring, 2012: 710 ). In this case nosotros (we) includes the entire Chilean audience, while the previous findings from political discourse studies indicate a tendency to exclude possible audience from the circle of this pronoun by politicians (Ivanova, 2013). Similarly to this, the deictic Ustedes (you, plural) also includes public in its scope, therefore, implicating it in policy decisions (Mulderring, 2012).

115
paper corpusSignostxt418 - : Prior, P. (2009). From speech genres to mediated multimodal genre systems: Bakhtin, Voloshinov, and the question of writing . En C. Bazerman, A. Bonini & D. Figueiredo (Eds.), Genre in a changing world (pp. 17-34). Fort Collins, CO: The WAC Clearinghouse and Parlor Press [en línea]. Disponible en: [71]http://wac.colostate.edu/books/genre/chapter2.pdf [ [72]Links ]

116
paper corpusSignostxt564 - : Bazzanella, C., Caffi, C. & Sbisà, M. (1991). Scalar dimension of illocutionary force. En I. Zagar (Ed.), Speech acts: Fiction or reality ? Proceedings of the International Conference, Ljubljana, Yugoslavia, November 15, 1990 (pp. 63-76). Ljubljana: Institute for Social Sciences. [ [123]Links ]

117
paper corpusSignostxt453 - : ^2 ^[165]Periñán and Mairal (2011) explain the methodology behind the creation of the Ontology of FunGramKB. For reasons of space, we shall only provide a brief explanation of this core component. The Ontology of FunGramKB is divided into three separate, albeit interrelated, subontologies in which the metaconcepts #ENTITIES, #EVENTS, and #QUALITIES respectively arrange in cognitive dimensions the following parts of speech: (i ) nouns, (ii) verbs, and (iii) adjectives. This type of organization stems from the fact that subsumption or IS-A is the only taxonomic relation permitted in the knowledge base.This contrasts with the approach adopted in FrameNet, for example, in which several frame-to-frame relations are posited (see ^[166]Ruppenhofer et al., 2010). These, however, have been shown to be problematic for NLP as far as reasoning is concerned (see ^[167]Ovchinnikova, Vieu, Oltramari, Borgo & Alexandrov, 2010). Conceptual, lexical and grammatical information is available through the NLP

118
paper corpusSignostxt266 - : Por otra parte, y siguiendo a Lakoff y Johnson (1980), estas dos metáforas conceptuales pueden ser vistas como 'metáforas convencionalizadas', y estos tipos de "metaphors are deeply entrenched and hence well know and widely used in a speech community" (Kövecses, 2002: 248 ). De acuerdo a Fillmore (1982) y Lakoff (1994), dado cierto marco conceptual, el mecanismo semántico generativo de importar-exportar significado entre los dominios marcará siempre la comprensión del concepto o los conceptos involucrados, tal como muestra el ejemplo que se ha analizado. Desde una perspectiva retórica, el hablante podrá enfatizar ciertos aspectos que el marco conceptual contiene, y sus preferencias podrían proyectar los tipos recurrentes de apoyos-marcos sociales respecto de los que el hablante es parte.

119
paper corpusSignostxt532 - : El análisis presentado en este documento se centrará en el estudio de movimientos argumentativos analíticamente relevantes, es decir, “those speech acts that (at least potentially) play a role in the process of resolving a difference of opinión” (nuestra traducción: “aquellos actos de habla que (al menos potencialmente) desempeñan un papel en el proceso de resolver una diferencia de opinión”) (van Eemeren & Grootendorst, 2004: 73 ). En particular, para el presente estudio, solo las discusiones que cumplen los tres criterios siguientes fueron seleccionadas para el análisis: 1) una diferencia de opinión entre padres e hijos sobre un problema relacionado con las reglas y normas de los padres se hace evidente en el discurso: ‘¿Puede el niño usar la goma para borrar el dibujo?’; 2) al menos un punto de vista utlizado por uno de los dos padres es cuestionado verbalmente por uno o más niños, o viceversa: (NIÑO) ‘Sí, quiero intentar’ - (PADRE) ‘No, no puedes’; 3) al menos uno de los

120
paper corpusSignostxt362 - : Abstract: This article analyzes the reported speech use in Santiago de Chile from a sociolinguistic point of view. For this purpose, we studied the discursive sequences in which they are used in 54 interviews that form part of the Sociolinguistic Corpus PRESEEA in Santiago, Chile. Studies on the polyphony of language (Ducrot, 1986; Reyes, 1993) and sociolinguistic analysis of discourse (Silva-Corvalán, 2001; Serrano, 2006) were consulted for the conceptual framework of this research. On the one hand, the analysis considers the reported speech as a sociolinguistic variable case, in a broad sense of the concept, with two values or general forms: direct speech and indirect speech, and with different subtypes or specific variations according to different levels or degrees of reformulation and detachment of the subjects from the utterance . Consequently, socio-demographic factors of subjects were correlated with the use of these variants, which were shown to have a relative impact on their

121
paper corpusSignostxt336 - : About 70 lexical functions have been identified in (Mel’čuk, 1996); each is associated with a particular meaning according to which it receives its name. The name of a lexical function is an abbreviated Latin word whose semantic content is closest to the meanings of this lexical function. Using the above notation, the collocation dar un paseo, lit. give a walk, is represented as Oper1(paseo) = dar where ‘Oper’ is from Latin operari (do, carry out); the argument, or the keyword of this lexical function is paseo; its value is dar; the subscript 1 stores information concerning the syntactical structure of utterances where the keyword of Oper1 (paseo) is used together with its value (dar) and where the first argument of paseo (Agent) is lexicalized in speech as the grammatical subject: Mi abuela (Agent ) da un paseo por este parque cada sábado, My grandma takes a walk in this park every Saturday. Other collocations that are isomorphic to dar un paseo can be represented likewise, and, in fact,

122
paper corpusSignostxt382 - : The senator begins his speech according to the rules of this activity type, i.e. by addressing the President of the meeting and the honourable colleagues. Next, he sets the type of speech that he is going to make, i.e. a free speech, unrehearsed and from the heart, underlining that his speech will not seek conflict: ‘We can talk as a family’ .

123
paper corpusSignostxt382 - : After this rather predictable and clichéd beginning, he deals with serious topics such as injustice, the mafia, the National Bank, unemployment, corruption – recurrent topics on his agenda. His strategy is to attack his opponents by giving concrete examples of facts and names from the opposition. The attitude that he manages to convey is that of a true fighter for justice, ready to make sacrifices in the name of the ‘public good. What is more, CVT delivers his speech in a threatening and solemn tone:

124
paper corpusSignostxt451 - : Correct pronunciation is a very important aspect of second language (L2) acquisition, indispensable not only for speech generation but also for adequate listening comprehension because the articulatory and auditory systems are interrelated: a learner is hardly able to recognize a sound s/he has never produced since it is absent in the first language or L1 (^[25]Levis, 2005 ). However, less accented speech generation and perfect listening comprehension are included in the requirements for some jobs, for instance, operators in call centers, so it is not a rare case that a learner may need more effective training in pronunciation (^[26]Hunter & Hachimi, 2012; ^[27]Lockwood, 2012).

125
paper corpusSignostxt464 - : Oral performance in a second language (L2) can be measured in terms of complexity, accuracy and fluency, thus offering an overview of L2 learning. Focusing on the cognitive development of L2 learning, particularly from the point of view of Processing Theory and the Speech Production Model, limited attentional resources hinder simultaneous attention to the variables studied, namely: Performance components of complexity, accuracy and fluency in semi spontaneous oral monologues at different levels of English proficiency (A2, B2 and C1 ). Complexity was measured in terms of lexical range (as D score, best fit for lexical range) and grammatical complexity (as the average length of words in each clause, and number of clauses in each Analysis of Speech (AS) unit). Accuracy was measured as the proportion of AS units free from errors and percentage of error-free clauses. Fluency was measured by calculating the phonation-time ratio and the mean length of pauses. Regarding trends in L2 language

126
paper corpusSignostxt464 - : Con respecto al análisis del discurso oral, ^[45]Foster, Tonkyn y Wigglesworth (2000) sugieren la unidad de Análisis del Habla (the analysis of speech unit /AS-unit): Un enunciado del hablante, compuesto por una cláusula independiente o unidad subclausal, junto con cualquier otra cláusula subordinada, asociada a cualquiera de las anteriores . En esta investigación, se hace referencia a dicha unidad de análisis como ‘unidad AH’.

127
paper corpusSignostxt313 - : In an analysis carried out in the early 1980's regarding the linguistic behaviour of Romanian speakers, Pietreanu (1984) noticed that using salutation formulas was not only a matter of individual freedom, but also a matter of adhering to the norms of behaviour that exist in a particular linguistic community at a certain moment. What is more, salutation formulas must take into account "the specific semantics of the situations in which a formula is actually used" (Pietreanu, 1984: 24). As a speech act, a greeting is defined as "communicative behaviour, either gestural or verbal (or both), that has a certain significance for a social micro- or macro-group, by means of which attention is paid, respect or politeness is shown to a person or to a group" (Pietreanu, 1984: 29 ).

128
paper corpusSignostxt579 - : The function ORIENTING THE STUDENTS was present in just over a third of the lectures (36%). Its most common subfunction is ‘greeting the students. This speech act is commonly realized through a restricted number of fixed phrases, such as:

129
paper corpusSignostxt141 - : This article is about the use of spontaneous gestures in interaction and, more precisely, about the repetition of the interlocutor's gestures. The analysis of four instances of gesture repetition found in two dyadic discussions with Spanish native speakers shows that this discursive activity operates in a way similar to verbal repetition. That is, the recipient can simply reproduce the interlocutor's gesture (retake) or recycle it by introducing various modifications (reformulation). All four instances of gesture repetition studied were performed by the same participant, who behaved almost like an interviewer with her partners. Gesture repetition played a part in facilitating the partners' speech in two areas: the negotiation of meaning (ideational dimension of language ), and the conduct of interaction (discursive dimension of language). More importantly, gesture repetition is an indicator that speakers pay attention to the interlocutors' gestures, confirming the communicative purpose of

130
paper corpusSignostxt141 - : De Ruiter, J.P. & Wilkins D.P. (1998). The synchronization of gesture and speech in Dutch and Arrente (an Australian Aboriginal language): a cross-cultural comparison . En S. Santi, I. Guaïtella, C. Cavé, & G. Konopczynski (Eds.), Oralité et gestualité. Communication multimodale, interaction (pp. 603-607). Paris: L'Harmattan. [ [86]Links ]

131
paper corpusSignostxt209 - : “There is a constant interplay between these two overarching characteristics of academic speech, which is by nature an information-rich genre, but in which interaction between the participants is also of paramount importance, and the formulaic expression identified here serve to highlight these dual pragmatic features” (Simpson, 2004: 60 ).

Evaluando al candidato speech:


2) acts: 32
5) linguistic: 19 (*)
8) teacher: 15
9) interaction: 14 (*)
10) speakers: 14 (*)
12) discourse: 14 (*)
14) native: 13
18) request: 12 (*)
19) oral: 12 (*)
20) learners: 12 (*)

speech
Lengua: eng
Frec: 2353
Docs: 804
Nombre propio: 21 / 2353 = 0%
Coocurrencias con glosario: 7
Frec. en corpus ref. en eng: 180
Puntaje: 7.681 = (7 + (1+7.3037807481771) / (1+11.2008986050384)));
Rechazado: muy disperso; muy común;

Referencias bibliográficas encontradas sobre cada término

(Que existan referencias dedicadas a un término es también indicio de terminologicidad.)
speech
: 1. Bakhtin, M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
: 13.Borzone de Manrique, Ana María. 1976. Acoustic analysis of [i, u] in the Spanish diphtongs. Language and Speech 19, 2. 121-127.
: 20.Dellwo, Volker. 2009. Choosing the right rate normalization method for measurements of speech rhythm. s.p. comunicación personal. University College London.
: 23.Grabe, Esther y Ee Ling Low. 2002. Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. En Carlos Gussenhoven y Natasha Warner (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7. 515-546. Berlín: Mouton.
: 26.Gut, Ulrike y Jan-Torsten Milde. 2002. The prosody of Nigerian English. En Bernard Bel y Isabelle Marlien (eds.), Speech Prosody, 367-370.
: 3. Smoothness pertains to the ease or regularity (^[40]Housen, Kuiken, & Vedder, 2012) with which a speaker can deliver spoken content. This component regulates the common notion of fluency as the continuity of the verbalisation of a speech (^[41]Koponen & Riggenbash, 2000).
: 37.O’Rourke, Erin. 2008 a. Correlating speech rhythm in Spanish: Evidence from two Peruvian dialects. En Joyce Bruhn de Garavito y Elena Valenzuela (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, 276-287. Somerville, Massachusetts: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
: 5. Searle, John. 1969. Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
: 8.Levelt, Willem. 1992. Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations. Cognition, 42. 1-22.
: ------; & Jacobs, S. (1980). Structure of conversational argument: Pragmatic bases for the enthymeme. Quarterly Journal of Speech 66, 251-265.
: ------; (1979). Expression and Meaning: Studies in the Theory of Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .
: 10. Firth, John. 1966. The tongues of men and speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: 11. Cohen, A. D. (1996). Speech acts. In S. L. Mckay and N. H. Hornberger (Eds.), Sociolinguistics and Language Teaching (pp. 383-420). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
: 11. Manes, J. y Wolfson, N. (1981). The compliment formula. En F. Coulmas (coord.), Conversational routine: Explorations in standardized communication situations and prepatterned speech (pp. 116-132). La Haya: Mouton.
: 11. Olshtain, E. y Cohen, A. D. (1991). Teaching speech act behavior to nonnative speakers. En M. Celce-Murcia (coord.), Teaching English as a second or foreign language (pp. 154-165). Boston, MA: Newbury House.
: 11.Hardin-Jones, Mary y David Jones. 2005. Speech production of preschoolers with cleft palate. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal 42. 7-13.
: 12. Cohen, A. D. (2004). Assessing speech acts in a second language. In D. Boxer and A.D. Cohen (Eds.), Studying Speaking to Inform Second Language Learning (pp. 302-327). Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
: 12. Dickerson, L. (1974). “Internal and External Patterning of Phonological Variability in the Speech of Japanese Learners of English”. Tesis Doctoral. University of Illinois.
: 12. Goldfield, Beverly. 1993. Noun bias in maternal speech to one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 20. 85-100.
: 12. Haverkate, H. (1988). Politeness strategies in verbal interaction: an analysis of directness and indirectness in speech acts. Semiotica, 71(1-2), 59-71.
: 12. Klima, Edward y Ursula Bellugi. 1966. Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. En John Lyons y Robert Wales (eds.), Psycholinguistics papers, 183-219. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
: 13. Calsamiglia, H. y López Ferrero, C. (2003). Role and position of scientific voices: Reported speech in the media. Discourse Studies, 5 ( 2), 147-173.
: 13. Jucker, A. (2009). Speech act research between armchair, field and laboratory: The case of compliments. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 1611–1635.
: 14. Cameron, R. (1998). A variable syntax of speech, gesture and sound effect: Direct quotations in Spanish. Language Variation and Change, 10, 43-83.
: 14. Levy, Yuri. 1999. Early metalinguistic competence: Speech monitoring and repair behavior. Developmental Psychology 35, 3. 822-834.
: 14. Liberman, A. (1957). Some results of research on speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, (29), 117-123.
: 14.Laver, John y Janet Beck. 2007. Vocal profile analysis scheme – VPAS. Edinburgh: Queen Margareth University College-QMUC, Speech Science Research Centre.
: 15. Hickmann, Maya. 1993. The boundaries of reported speech in narrative discourse: some developmental aspects. En John Lucy (ed.), Reflexive language. Reported speech and metapragmatics, 63-90. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 15. Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 16. Castellanos, Isabel. 1990. Grammatical structure, historical development and religious usage of Afro-Cuban Bozal speech. Folklore Forum 23, 1- 2. 57-84.
: 16. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. (2007). Natural speech vs. elicited data: A comparison of natural and role play requests in Mexican Spanish. Spanish in Context, 4(2), 159-185.
: 17. Gilbert, J. B. (1993). Clear speech: Pronunciation & listening comprehension in North American English. Student’s book, 2^nd. Ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.
: 17. Haas, M. (1944). Men's and women's speech in Koasati. Language, 20, 142-149.
: 17. Manes, J. &Wolfson, N. (1981).The compliment formula. En F. Coulmas (Ed.), Conversational Routine: Explorations in Standardized Communication Situations and Prepatterned Speech (pp. 116- 132). La Haya, Holanda: Mouton Publishers.
: 18. Franch Bou, P. and Lorenzo-Dus, N. (2008). Natural versus elicited data in cross-cultural speech act realisation: The case of requests in Peninsular Spanish and British English. Spanish in Context, 5(2), 246-277.
: 18. Hengeveld, K., Rijkhoff, J. y Siewierska, A. (2004). Parts of Speech Systems and Word Order. Journal of Linguistics, 40(3), 527-570.
: 19. Millard, Tomy Lynn Richman. 2001. Different cleft conditions, facial appearance, and speech: Relationship to psychological variables. Cleft Palate- Craniofacial Journal 38, 1. 68-75.
: 2. Bakhtin, M. (1986). The problem of speech genres and other late essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
: 2. Baldwin, T. (2005). Deep lexical acquisition of verb- particle constructions. Computer Speech and Language, 19(1), 398–414.
: 2. Biber, Douglas. 1988. Variation across speech and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 2. Blum-Kulka, S. (1982). Learning to say what you mean in a second language: A study of the speech act performance of learners of Hebrew as a second language. Applied Linguistics, 3, 29-59.
: 20. Fue Aijmer (1985: 385), aunque muy de pasada, el que se refirió a esta función al ocuparse del elemento de final de serie or something, que podría funcionar “as a 'softener' mitigating the force of the speech act”.
: 20. Lucy, John. 1993. Reflexive language and the human disciplines. En John Lucy (ed.), Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics, 9-32. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 20. Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 20.Peterson-Falzone, Sally; Mary Hardin-Jones y Michael Karnell. 2001. Cleft palate speech. St. Louis: Mosby.
: 21. Myers, G. (1992). ''In this paper we report...'': Speech acts and scientific facts, Journal of Pragmatics, 17, 295-313.
: 21. Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge, MS: Cambridge University Press.
: 22.Tabensky, Alexis. 2001. Gesture and speech rephrasings in conversation.Gesture 1, 2. 213-235.
: 23. Jakobson, R., G. Fant y M. Halle. (1952). Preliminaries to Speech Analysis. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
: 24.Willems, Roel; Asli Özyürek y Peter Hagoort. 2005. when language meets action: the neural integration of gesture and speech. Cerebral Cortex 17. 2322-2333.
: 25. Rowland, Caroline; Julian Pine; Elena Lieven y Anna Theakston 2003. Determinants of the order of acquisition of wh- questions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. Journal of Child Language 30. 609-635.
: 25. Tardif, Twila; Marilyn Shatz y Letitia Naigles. 1997. Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 24. 535-565.
: 27. Jurafsky, Daniel y Martin, James. 1999. Speech and language processing: An introduction to natural language processing, computational linguistics, and speech recognition. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
: 27. Munro,M. & T. Derwin. (1995). “Foreign Accent, Comprehensibility, and Intelligibility in the Speech of Second Language Learners”. Language Learning, 45:1. 73-97.
: 28. Williams, Dale y Susan Dietrich. 1999. Effects of speech and language disorders on raters' perceptions. Journal of Communication Disorders 1, 29. 1-12.
: 3. Bach, Kent y Robert Harnish. 1979. Linguistic communication and speech acts. Cambridge, MA: The M.I.T. Press.
: 30. Olshtain, Elite y LioraWeinbach. 1987. Complaints.Astudy of speech act behavior among native and nonnative speakers of Hebrew. En Jeff Verschueren y Marcella Bertucelli Papi (eds.), The pragmatic perspective, 195-208. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
: 32- Miller, C. R. (1984). Genre and social action. Quarterly Journal of Speech,
: 32. Grice, P. (1975). Logic and conversation. En P. Cole y J. Morgan (coords.), Speech acts: Syntax and semantics (pp. 41-58). Nueva York: Academic Press.
: 33 Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: 33. Lipski, John. 1985. The speech of the negros congos of Panama: Creole Spanish vestiges? Hispanic Linguistics 2. 23-47.
: 33. Searle, John R. 1975. Indirect speech acts. En Peter Cole y JamesMorgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts, 59-82. New York: Academic Press.
: 36. Lipski, John. 1989. The speech of the NEGROS CONGOS of Panama. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
: 36. Maeshiba, N., Yoshinaga, N., Kasper, G., & Ross, S. (1996). Transfer and proficiency in interlanguage apologizing. In S. M. Gass & J. Neu (Eds.), Speech acts across cultures. Challenges to communication in a second language (pp. 155–187). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
: 36. van der Houwen, F. (1998). Organizing discourse. Direct and indirect speech in Mexican Spanish. Linguistics in the Netherlands, 15, 123-134.
: 38. Wierzbicka,Anna. 1985. Different cultures, different language, different speech acts. Journal of Pragmatics 9. 145-178.
: 39. Dumas, B. K. y Lighter, J. (1978). Is slang a word for linguists? American Speech, Durham (NC): Duke University Press, LII, 1, 5-17.
: 4. Bebout, Linda yArthur Bradford. 1992. Cross-cultural attitudes toward speech disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35. 45-52.
: 4. Brotherton, Patricia. 1976. Aspects of the relationship between speech production.Hesitation behaviour and social class. Trabajo inédito. Melbourne: University of Melbourne.
: 42. Olshtain, E., & Blum-Kulka, S. (1985). Degree of approximation: Nonnative reactions to native speech act behavior. In S. M. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 303-325). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
: 46.Ramus, Franck, Marina Nespor y Jacques Mehler.1999. Correlates of linguistic rhythm in speech. Cognition 73. 265-292.
: 47. Jucker, A.H. y Taavitsainen, I. (2000). Diachronic speech act analysis: Insults from flyting to flaming. Journal of Historical Pragmatics, 1(1): 67-95.
: 5. Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). In M. Holquist & C. Emerson (Eds.), Speech genres and other late essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
: 5. Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. En P. Cole y J. L. Morgan (Comps.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech Acts (pp. 41-58). Nueva York: Academic Press.
: 5. Orange, J.B; Lubinski R y Higginbotham. (1996). Conversational Repair by Individuals with Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. Volumen 39, 881-895.
: 6. Colin, B. (2005). Learning about the meaning of verb– particle constructions from corpora. Computer Speech and Language, 19(1), 467–478.
: 6. Delgado, V. ( 1994). Politeness in language: Directive speech acts in U.S. English, and Colombian and Castilian Spanish. Tesis doctoral no publicada, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Estados Unidos.
: 6. Dorian, Nancy. 1982. Defining the speech community to include its working margins. En Suzann Romaine (ed.) Sociolinguistic variation in speech communities, 25-33. Londres: Arnold.
: 7.Gibbon, Fiona; Lucy Ellis y Lisa Crampin. 2004. Articulatory placement for /t/, /d/, /k/ and /g/ targets in school age children with speech disorders associated with cleft–palate. Clinical Linguitics & Phonetics 18. 391-404.
: 72. Olshtain, E. & Cohen, A. (1983). Apology: A speech act set. In Wolfson, N. & Judd, E. (Eds.), So-ciolinguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 18-35). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
: 8. Ripich, D.N y Terrell, B.Y. (1988). Patterns of Discourse Cohesion and Coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 53: 8-15.
: Aceto, M. (2002b). Ethnic personal names and multiple identities in Anglophone caribbean speech communities in Latin America. Language in Society, 31, 577-608.
: Agirre, A. y Edmonds, Ph. (Eds.). 2007. Word sense disambiguation algorithms and applications. Text, Speech and Language Technology, Vol. 33. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer-Verlag.
: Aguilar, M. (2008). Metadiscourse in academic speech: A relevance-theoretic approach. Bern: Peter Lang.
: Ahangar, A., Jafarzadeh Fadaki, S. M. & Sehhati, A. (2016). The study of lexical relations device in speech of elderly Alzheimer patients and non-patients. Journal of Fundamentals of Mental Health, 18(1), 22-28.
: Ahmed, S., Haigh, A. H., Jager, C. A. & Garrard, P. (2013). Connected speech as a marker of disease progression in autopsy-proven Alzheimer´s disease. Brain, 136, 3727-3737.
: Alston, W. (1994). Illocutionary acts and linguistic meaning. En L. Tsohatzidis (coord.), Foundations of speech act theory: Philosophical and linguistic perspectives (pp. 29-49). Londres: Routledge.
: Amengual, M. (2012). Interlingual influence in bilingual speech: Cognate status effect in a continuum of bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 517-530. [100]https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000460
: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) (1980). Committee on Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, Definitions for communicative disorders and differences, ASHA, 22,317-8.
: Anderson, R. (1996). Assessing the Grammar of Spanish-Speaking Children: A Comparison of Two Procedures. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 27, (pp. 333-344).
: Another study that explores the advantages of using language to dialogue with the self was conducted by Appel and Lantolf (1994) who examined the role of speaking in what Vygotsky calls inner speech.
: Aoyama, K., Flege, J. E., Guion, S. G., Akahane-Yamada, R. & Yamada, T. (2004). Perceived phonetic dissimilarity and L2 speech learning: The case of Japanese/r/and English/l/and/r. Journal of Phonetics, 32(2), 233-250.
: Archibald, L., y Gathercole, S. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49(5), 970-983.
: Ardila, A. & Ramos, E. (2007) Bilingualism and cognition: the good, the bad and the ugly of bilingualism. En A. Ardila & E. Ramos (Eds.), Speech and language disorders in bilinguals (pp. 213-234). New York: Nova Science Publishers.
: Ardila, A. Y Ramos, E. (2007). Speech and Language Disorders in Bilinguals. New York: Nova Science Publishers Inc.
: Arias, J. P., Becerra, N., & Vivanco, H. (2010). Automatic intonation assessment for computer aided language learning. Speech Communication, 52, 254-267.
: Arvaniti, A. (2012). The Usefulness of Metrics in the Quantification of Speech Rhythm.Journal of Phonetics, 40, 351-373.
: As we identified quoted speech throughout the episodes of the story, we discovered that these areas had the most evident use of punctuation. This confirmed what Ferreiro and Zucchermaglio (1996) mentioned about the early use of punctuation marks within texts.
: Asher, N. & Lascarides, A. (2001). Indirect speech acts. Synthese, 128, 183-228.
: Asia India Conversation of SL to Speech with Gestures [37]Rajaganapathy et al. (2015)
: Aslin, R. N. & Pisoni, D. B. (1980). Effects of early linguistic experience on speech discrimination by infants: A critique of. Child Development, 51(1), 107-112.
: Auerbach, E. (1999). Border Skirmshes: The power of literacies vs. the literacies of power. Keynote speech, Rocky Mountain TESOL Conference, Las Cruces, NM.
: Auza, A. & Roldán, M. (2003). Derivational Morphology Deficits in Mexican Children with SLI. Trabajo presentado en el Congreso de American Speech and Hearing Association. nov. 14 al 16, Chicago - IL, USA. ASHA.
: Azevedo, M. M. (1978). Identifying Spanish Interference in the Speech of Learners of Portuguese. The Modern Language Journal, 62(1, 2), 18-23.
: BAJTÍN, M. (1986). Speech Genres and Other Late Essays. C. Emerson y M. Holquist (ed.). Austin: Universidad de Texas.
: Bach, K. & Harnish, R. M. (1979). Linguistic Communication and Speech Acts. Cambridge (Mass): MIT Press.
: Bajtin, M. (1986). "The problem of speech genres". Speechgenresand otherlate essays. Austin, Estados Unidos: University ofTexasPress,60-102
: Bajtin, M. (1986). Speech Genres and other esays. Minneapolis: University of McGee.
: Baker, W., Eddington, D. & Nay, L. (2009). Dialect identification: The effects of region of origin and amount of experience. American Speech, 84(1), 48-71.
: Bakhtin, M. (1986). Response to a question from the novy mir editorial staff. En C. Emerson & M. Holquist (Eds.), Speech genres and other late essays (pp. 1). Austin: university of Texas.
: Bakhtin, M.M. (1986) Speech genres and other late essays (V.W. McGee, Trans.) Austin: University of Texas Press.
: Banfield, Ann. 1973. Narrative style and the grammar of direct and indirect speech, Foundations of Language, 10,1: 1-39.
: Banfield, Ann. 1978. Where epistemology, style, and grammar meet literary history: The development of represented speech and thought, New Literary History, 9,3: 415-454.
: Bara, B., Bosco, F. & Bucarelli, M. (1999). Simple and complex speech acts: What makes the difference within a developmental perspective. En M. Hahn & S. Stones (Eds.), Proceedings of the XXI Cognitive Science Society (pp. 55-60). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.
: Barbu, V. (2008). Hyponymy patterns: Semi-automatic extraction, evaluation and inter-lingual comparison. En P. Sojka, A. Horak, I. Kopecek & P. Karel (Eds.), Text, Speech and Dialogue (pp. 37-44). Berlin: Springer.
: Bard, E.G., R.C. Shillcock & G. Altmann. 1988. "the recognition of words after t^heIr acoustic offsets in spontaneous speech: Effects of subsequent context". Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 395-408.
: Baron, N. S. (1998). Letters by phone or speech by other means: The linguistics of email. Language and Communication, 18, 133-170 [en línea]. Disponible en: [137]https://doi.org/10.1016/S0271-5309(98)00005-6
: Barrett, R. (1999). Indexing polyphonous identity in the speech of African American drag queens. En M. Bucholtz, A. Liang & L. Sutton (Eds.), Reiventing identitities (pp. 313-332). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21, 205-224.
: Bazerman, C. (2003). Speech acts, genres, and activity systems: How texts organize activity and people. En C. Bazerman y P. Prior (Eds.), What writing does and how it does it (pp. 315-346). Londres y Nueva York: Routledge .
: Bazzanella, C., Caffi, C. & Sbisá, M. (1991). Scalar dimension of illocutionary force. En I. Zagar (Ed.), Speech acts: Fiction or reality? (pp. 63-76). Ljubljana: IPrA Distribution Center for Yugoslavia.
: Beckman, J. y Ayers, G. (1994). Guidelines for ToBI Labelling, version 2.0. Ms. and Accompanying Speech Materials. Columbus: The Ohio State University.
: Bedore, L. & Leonard, L. (2001) Grammatical Morphology Deficits in Spanish Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hering Research 44, (pp. 905-924).
: Bedore, L. & Leonard, L. B. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(2), 195-225.
: Behrens, Heike e Ulrike Gut. 2005. The relationship between prosodic and syntactic organization in early multiword speech, Journal of Child Language, 32: 1-34.
: Bergmann, K., Aksu, V. & Kopp, S. (2011). The relation of speech and gestures: Temporal Synchrony Follows Semantic Synchrony. Ponencia presentada en el 2nd Workshop on Gesture and Speech in Interaction, Bielefeld, Alemania.
: Bergsleithner, J. 2007. "The relationship among individual differences in working memory capacity, noticing, and L2 speech production". Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Florianópolis, Brazil: Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina.
: Berk, Laura. E. (1992). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. En R. M. Díaz y L. E. Berk (eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17-53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
: Berman, R. & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Cross-linguistic perspectives on the development of text-production abilities: Speech and writing. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 1-44.
: Bernstein Ratner, N. (1996). From 'Signal to Syntax': But what is the nature of the signal? In J.L. Morgan & K. Demuth (eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 135-150). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
: Bertinetto, P.M. & Bertini, C. (2008). On modeling the rhythm of natural languages.Proceedings of Speech Prosody Conference 2008, 427-430.
: Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realistic view of cross-language speech perception. En W. Strange, Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research (pp. 171-204). Nueva York: Timonium, MD: York Press, Inc.
: Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W. & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 345-360.
: Biber, D. (1988). "Variation across Speech and Writing" , Cambridge: CUP.
: Biber, D. (1988). Linguistic features: Algorithms and functions in variation across speech and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Biber, D., Conrad, S. & Cortés, V. (2003). Lexical bundles in speech and writing: An initial taxonomy. En A. Wilson, P. Rayson, & T. McEnery (Eds.), Corpus linguistics by the lune: A festschrift for Geoffrey Leech (pp. 71–92). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
: Biber, Douglas (1988). Variation across Speech and Writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Bird, J. Bishop, D.VM and Freeman N. H. (1995). Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 38 (2), 446-462. [96]https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3802.446
: Bishop, D. & Adams, C. (1992). Comprehension problems in children with specific language impairment: Literal and inferential meaning. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(1), 119-129.
: Bishop, D. & Leonard, L. (2001). Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome. Oxford: Psychology Press.
: Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1992).Comprehension problems in children with specific language impairment: Literal and inferential meaning. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 119-129.
: Bishop, Dorothy y Leonard, Laurence. (2001). Speech and language impairments in children: causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome. Oxford, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.
: Blake, J., Myszczyszyn, D., Jokel, A. & Bebiroglu, N. (2008). Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech. First Language, 28(2), 237-253. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723707087583
: Blum-Kulka, S. (1980). Learning to Say What You Mean in a Second Language: a Study of the Speech Act Performance of Learners of Hebrew as a Second Language. Applied Linguistics, 3(1), 29-59. Recuperado de [101]http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED195173.
: Blum-Kulka, S., & Olshtain, E. (1984). Requests and apologies: A cross-cultural study of speech act realization patterns (CCSARP) . Applied Linguistics, 5(3), 196-213. [110]https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/5.3.196
: Bosch, L., Figueras, M., Teixidó, M., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2013). Rapid gains in segmenting fluent speech when words match the rhythmic unit: evidence from infants acquiring syllable- timed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 106.
: Boschi, V., Catricala, E., Consonni, M., Chesi, C., Moro, A. & Cappa, S. F. (2017). Connected speech in neurodegenerative language disorders: A review. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 269.
: Bosma Smit, Ann. (1993). Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska Articulation Norms Project: Word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 931-947.
: Bowerman, M. (1982). Starting to talk worse: Clues to language acquisition from children's late speech errors, in S. Strauss & R. Stavy (Eds.), U-shaped behavioral growth. (pp. 101-146) Nueva York, USA: Academic Press.
: Boxer, D. & Pickering, L. (1995). Problems in the presentation of speech acts in ELT materials: the case of complaint. ELT Journal, 49(1), 44-58.
: Bradac, J. J. & Mulac, A. (1984). A molecular view of powerful and powerless speech styles: Attributional consequences of specific language features and communicator intentions. Communication Monographs, 51(4), 307-319.
: Bradac, J. J., Hemphill, M. R. & Tardy, C. H. (1981). Language style on trial: Effects of powerful and powerless speech upon judgments of victims and villains. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 45(4), 327-341.
: Bradlow, A. R., Pisoni, D. B., Akahane-Yamada, R. & Tohkura, Y. (1997). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101(4), 2299-2310.
: Brazil, D. (1995). A grammar of speech. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
: Brill, E. 1995. "Unsupervised learning of disambiguation rules for part of speech tagging". En Yarowsky, D. y Church, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Third Association for Computational Linguistics Workshop on Very Large Corpora. Cambridge: MA, pp. 1-13.
: Brinton, Bonnie e Martin Fujiki. 1982. A comparison of request-response sequences in the discourse of normal and language-disordered children, Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47: 57-62.
: Britain, D. (1991). Dialect and Space: A Geolinguistic Analysis of Speech Variables in the Fens [tesis de doctorado no publicada, University of Essex].
: Broca, P. (1965). Loss of speech, chronic softening and partial destruction of the left anterior lobe of the brain. En R.H. Wikins (compilador), Neurosurgical Classics (pp. 61- 68). Nueva York: Johnson Reprint Corporation.
: Brown, A., & Chen, J. (2013). Construal of manner in speech and gesture in Mandarin, English, and Japanese. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(4), 605-631.
: Brown, P. & Fraser, C. (1979). Speech as a marker of situation. En K. R. Scherer & H. Giles (Eds.), Social markers in speech (pp. 33–62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Bruner, Jerome S. 1975. The ontogenesis of speech acts, Journal of Child Language , 2: 1-19.
: Buac, M., Gross, M., & Kaushanskayaa, M. (2014). The role of primary caregiver vocabulary knowledge in the development of bilingual children’s vocabulary skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing, 57, 1804-1816.
: Bunn, L, Roy, E. & Elliot, D. (2007). Speech perception and motor control in children with Down syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 13(2), 262-275.
: Burkhardt, A. (Ed.) (1990). Speech acts, meaning and intentions: critical approaches to the philosophy of John R. Searle. Berlín: De Gruyter.
: Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. En D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and Gesture (pp. 235-258). Nueva York: Cambridge University Press.
: Butler, J. (1997). Excitable Speech. A Politic of the Performative. New York: Routledge.
: Butterworth, B. & Beattie, G. (1978). Gesture and silence as indicators of planning in speech. En R. Campbell & G. T. Smith (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language: Formal and experimental approaches (pp. 347-360). Nueva York: Plenum Press.
: Butterworth, B. (1989). Lexical access in speech production. En W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representation and process (pp. 108-135). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
: Butterworth, B., Beattie, G.W. (1978). Gesture and silence as indicators of planning in speech. En R. Campbell & P. Smith (Eds.), Recent Advances in the Psychology of Language: Formal and Experimental Approaches (pp. 347-360). New York: Plenum.
: COLE, P. (1975). Syntax and Semantics 3 Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press.
: Caldas-Coulthard, C. (2004). On reporting reporting: the representation of speech in factual and factional narratives. En M. Coulthard (Ed.), Advances in Written Text Analysis (pp. 295-308). Londres-Nueva York: Routledge.
: Calsamiglia, H. & López, C. (2003). Role and position of scientific voices: Reported speech in the media. Discourse Studies, 5(2), 147-173.
: Cameron, D., F. Mcalinden & K. Oleary (1988) Women in their Speech Communities. London: Longman.
: Cameron, R. (1998), “A variable syntax of speech, gesture, and sound effect: Direct quotations in Spanish”, Language Variation and Change , 10:1, pp. 43-83.
: Campbell, K. K. (1986). Style and content in the rhetoric of early afro-american feminists. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 72, 434-445.
: Campbell, Thomas; Dollaghan, Christine; Rockette, Howard y Paradise, Jack. 2003. "Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children", en Child Development 4 (2), pp. 346-357.
: Capone, N. C. & McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development: A review for clinicians and researchers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 173-186.
: Carreiras, M. & Clifton, C. (1993). Relative clause interpretation preferences in spanish and English. Language and Speech, 36, 353-372.
: Catford, J. C. (1964). Phonation types: the classification of some laryngeal components of speech production. En D. Abercrombie, D. B. Fry, P. A. D. MacCarthy, N. C. Scout & J. L. M. Trim (eds.), In honour of Daniel Jones (pp. 26-37). London, Longmans, Green and Co.
: Catts, Hugh; Fey, Marc; Tomblin, Bruce y Zhang, Xuyang. 2002. "A longitudinal investigation of reading out comes in children with language impairment", en Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 45, pp. 1142-1157.
: Centeno-Cortés, Beatriz y Jiménez, A. (2004). Problem solving task in a foreign language: The importance of the L1 in private speech thinking. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1, 7-35.
: Cera, M. L., Ortiz, K. Z., Bertolucci, P. H. F. & Minett, T. (2018). Phonetic and phonological aspects of speech in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology, 32(1), 88-102.
: Chaika, E. & Lambe, R. (1985). The locus of dysfunction in schizophrenic speech. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11(1), 8-15.
: Chambers, J. K. (1980). Linguistic variation and Chomsky's homogenous speech community. En M. Kinloch & A. B. House (Eds.), Papers from the fourth Annual Meeting of the Atlantic Provinces Linguistics Association (pp. 1-32). Fredericton: University of New Brunswick.
: Chang, Charles B. 2012. Rapid and multifaceted effects of second-language learning on first-language speech production, Journal of Phonetics, 40: 249-268.
: Chapman, R., Seung, H., Schwartz, S., & Kay-Raining Bird, E. (1998). Language Skills of Children and Adolescents With Down Syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,41, 861-873.
: Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2008). The sound of sarcasm. Speech Communication 50, 366-381.
: Chen, Miao-Tzu. (2006). An interlanguage study of the speech act of disagreement made by Chinese EFL Speakers in Taiwan. Unpublished Master's Thesis, Koashiung: National Sun Yatsen University.
: Chen, X. (2010). Discourse-grammatical features in L2 speech: A corpus-based contrastive study of Chinese advanced learners and native speakers of English. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
: Choudhury, Naseem y Benasich, April Anne. 2003. "A family aggregation study: the influence of family history and other risk factors on language development", en Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 46 (2), pp. 261-272.
: Church, R., Kelly, S. & Lynch, K. (2000). Immediate memory for mismatched speech and representational gesture across development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 151-174.
: Clark, H. (1979). Responding to indirect speech acts. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 430-477.
: Clements, G. N. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In J. Kingston, & M. E. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology I. Between the grammar and the physics of speech, (pp. 283-333). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
: Clift, R. (2006). Indexing stance: Reported speech as an interactional evidential. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 10(5), 569-95.
: Clift, R. (2007). Getting there first: non-narrative reported speech in interaction. En E. Holt & R. Clift (Eds.), Reporting Talk: Reported Speech in Interaction (pp. 120-149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Clopper, Cinthia y Pisoni, David (2005) “Perception on dialect variation”. En The Handbook of speech perception. Eds., David Pisoni y Robert E. Remez. Australia: Blackwell, Publishing, Ltd.
: Cohen, A. (1996). Investigating the production of speech act sets. In S. M. Gass & J. Neu (Eds.), Speech acts across cultures: Challenges to communication in a second language (pp. 21-43). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
: Cole, R.A. & Jakimik, J. (1980). A model of speech perception. In R.A. Cole (ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech (pp. 133-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
: Coll, Magdalena. 2013b. Representation of Charrúa Speech in 19th Century Uruguayan Literature, en L. Callahan (ed.), Spanish and Portuguese Across Time, Place, and Borders. Studies in Honour of Milton M. Azevedo, Nueva York, Palgrave Macmillan: 110-131.
: Collier, R . y Hart, J. T. 1975. "The role of intonation in speech perception", en International Review of Applied Linguistics 13, pp. 293-308.
: Colozzo, P., Gillam, R., Wood, M., Schnell, R. & Johnston, R. (2011). Journal Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 1609-1627. Doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0247).
: Conrad, S. & Biber, D. (2001). Adverbial marking of stance in speech and writing. En S. Hunston & G. Thompson (Eds.), Evaluation in Text. Authorial Stance and the Construction of Discourse (pp. 56-73). Oxford: Oxford University Press .
: Conti-Ramsden, G. & Durkin, K. (2008). Language and independence in adolescents with and without a history of Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51(1), 70-83.
: Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistics markers in young children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1029-1037.
: Cook, H. (2001). Why can't learners of Japanese as a foreign language distinguish polite from impolite speech styles? In Kenneth R. Rose & Gabriele Kasper (Eds.) Pragmatics in Language Teaching. (pp. 80-102). New York: Cambridge University Press.
: Cooper, W. & Paccia-Cooper, J. (1980). Syntax and speech. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
: Cooper, W.E. y Klouda, G.V. 1988. Contrastive stress, intonation, and stuttering frequency. Language and Speech, 31, 1, 3-20.
: Cordella, M., Large, H. y Pardo, V. (1995). Complimenting behavior in Australian English and Spanish speech. Multilingua, 14(3), 235-252.
: Corder (1981) establishes four steps to carry out error analysis research: collection of learners' speech sample, identification of errors, the description of the errors that have been identified and finally the explanation of learner's errors.
: Corley, M., & Stewart, O. W. (2008). Hesitation disfluencies in spontaneous speech: The meaning of um. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 589-602.
: Correia, S., Butler, J., Vigário, M. & Frota, S. (2015). A Stress “Deafness” Effect in European Portuguese. Language and Speech, 58(1), 48-67.
: Coulthard, M. (1994). On the use of corpora in the analysis of forensic texts. Forensic Linguistics. The International Journal of Speech Language and the Law, 1(1), 27-43.
: Cowie, R. & Cornelius, R.R. (2003). Describing the emotional states that are expressed in speech. Speech communication, 40(1-2), 5-32.
: Croft, W. (2000). Parts of Speech as Language Universals and as Language-Particular Categories. En P. Vogel, & B. Comrie (eds.), Approaches to the typology of word classes (pp. 65-102). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [119]https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110806120.65
: Cunningham, J., Nicol, T., Zecker, S. G., Bradlow, A. y Kraus, N. 2001. Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in children with learning problems: deficits and strategies for improvement. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(5), 758- 767.
: Cutler, A. & Carter, D. M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech & Language, 2(34-35), 133-142.
: Cutler, A. (1996). Prosody and the word boundary problem. In J.L. Morgan & K. Demuth (eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 87-99). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
: Cutler, A. (2008). Lexical Stress. En Pisoni, & D. B. R. E. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 264-289). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
: Cutler, A. y D.M. Carter. 1987. the predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech and Language, 2, 133-142.
: Cutler, A. y S. Butterfield. 1992. "Rhythmic Cues to Speech Segmentation: Evidence from Juncture Misperception". Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 218-236.
: Cutler, A., & Foss, D. J. (1977). On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 20(1), 1-10.
: Cutler, A., Dahan, D. y Van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: a literature review. Language and speech, 40(2), 141-201.
: Cárdenes-Hagan, E., Carlson, C. D., & Pollard-Durodola, S. D. (2007). The cross-linguistic transfer of early literacy skills: The role of initial L1 and L2 skills and language of instruction. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 12(3), 249-259.
: D'Alessandro, C. & Mertens, P 1995. "Automatic pitch contour stylization using a model of tonal perception". Computer Speech & language, 9, 257-288.
: Danly, M y Shapiro, B. 1982. Speech prosody in Broca's aphasia. Brain and Language, 16, 171-190.
: De Cock, S. (1998). A recurrent word combination approach to the study of formulae in the speech of native and non–native speakers of English. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 3(1), 59– 80.
: De Lisi, LE. (2001). Speech disorder in schizophrenia: Review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophr Bull. 27(3): 481-496.
: De Ruiter, J. P. (1998). Gesture and speech production. Tesis doctoral, Universidad de Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Países Bajos.
: De Ruiter, J. P., (2000). The production of gesture and speech. En D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and Gesture (pp. 284-311). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech. The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.
: Dellwo, V. (2006). Rhythm and speech rate: a variation coefficient for deltaC. En P. Karnowski & I. Szigeti (Eds.),Language and language-processing(pp. 231-241). Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang.
: Dewaele, J. M. & Furnham, A. (2000). Personality and speech production: A pilot study of second language learners. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(2), 355-365.
: Dewaele, J.-M. (2004c). The Emotional Force of Swearwords and Taboo Words in the Speech of Multilinguals. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 25(2/3), 204-222.
: Dilley, L. & Pitt, M. (2007). A Study of Regressive Place Assimilation in Spontaneous Speech and its Implications for Spoken Word Recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122(4), 2340-2353.
: Diniz, L. (2010). Suggestions and recommendations in academic speech. Corpus-linguistic applications. In S. T. Gries, S. Wulff & M. Davies (Eds.), Current studies, new directions (pp. 169-181). Amsterdam: Rodopi .
: Dollaghan, C., Campbell, T., Paradise, J., Feldman, H., Janosky, J. & Pitcairn, D. (1999). Maternal Education and Measures of Early Speech and Language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, (pp. 1432-1443).
: Down, B., & Tonn, M. B. (1993). 'Feminine style' and political judgment in the rhetoric of Ann Richards. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 79, 286-302.
: Dromey, C., Silveira, J., y Sandor, P. 2005. Recognition of affective prosody by speakers of English as a first or foreign language. Speech Communication, 47, 3, 351-359.
: Du Bois, J. (1986). Self-evidence and ritual speech. En W. Chafe & J. Nichols (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology (pp. 313-336). Norwood: Albex.
: Duffy, J. R., Strand, E.A. & Josephs, K. A. (2014). Motor speech disorders associated with primary progressive aphasia, Aphasiology, 28(8-9), 1004-1017.
: Dupoux, E., y Peperkam, S. (2002). Fossil markers of language development: Phonological deafnesses in adult speech processing. En J. Durand y B. Laks, Phonetics, Phonology, and Cognition (pp. 168-190). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: Díaz, Rafael M. y Berk, Laura E. (eds.). (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
: Díaz, Rafael M., Padilla, K.A. y Weathersby, E.K. (1991). The effects of bilingualism on preschoolers' private speech. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6, 377-393.
: Díaz-Campos, M. A. (2001). Acquisition of Phonological Structure y Sociolinguistic Variables: a Quantitative Analysis of Spanish Consonant Weakening in Venezuelan Children’s Speech. Ohio: Ohio State University.
: D’Odorico, Laura, Mirco Fasolo e Daniela Marchione. 2009. The prosody of early multi-word speech: Word order and its intonational realization in the speech of Italian children, Enfance, 3: 317-327.
: EADY, S. J., COOPER, W.E., KLOUDA.- MUELLER, P.R- & LOTTS, D.W., (1986). "Acoustical characteristics of sentential focus: narrow vs. broad and single vs. dual focus environments" , in Language and Speech 29, 3, 233-251
: Edwards, J. & Jacobsen, M. (1987). Standard and Regional Standard Speech: Distinctions and Similarities. Language in Society 16 (3). 369-379.
: Eisenberg, S. & Guo, L. (2013). Differentiating children with and without language impairment based on grammaticality. Language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 44(1), 20-31.
: Erickson, B., Lind, E. A., Johnson, B. C. & O’Barr, W. M. (1978). Speech style and impression formation in a court setting: The effects of powerful and powerless speech. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(3), 266-279.
: Errington, J. Joseph (1985). "On the nature of the sociolinguistic sign: Describing the Javanese speech levels". En Elizabeth Mertz and Richard J. Parmentier (eds.) Semiotic Mediation. Orlando, Florida: Academic Press, 287310.
: Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (2009). Hymes on speech socialization. Text and Talk, 29, 245-256. [176]https://doi.org/10.1515/TEXT.2009.013
: Eskenazi, M. (2009). An overview of spoken language technology for education. Speech Communication,51(10), 832-844.
: Esling, J. H. & Harris, J. G. (2005). States of the glottis: An articulatory phonetic model based on laryngoscopic observations. En W. Hardcastle. & J. Beck (eds.), A Figure of Speech: A Festschrift for John Laver (pp. 347-83). Mahwah, Nj: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
: Estellés-Arguedas, M. (2015), “Expressing evidentiality through prosody? Prosodic voicing in reported speech in Spanish colloquial conversations”, Journal of Pragmatics, 85, pp. 138-154.
: Esteve-Gibert, N. & Prieto P. (2014). Infants temporally coordinate gesture-speech combinations before they produce their first words. Speech Communication, 57, 301-316.
: Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic theory of speech production with calculations based on X-ray studies of Russian articulations. The Hague: Mouton.
: Fant, G. 1970. Acoustic theory of speech production, 2ª ed., Paris, Mouton.
: Fant, Gunnar. 1960. Acoustic theory of speech production. With calculations based on X-ray studies of Russian articulations, Den Haag, Mouton
: Fava, E. (2002). Editor's introduction. En E. Fava (Ed.), Clinical linguistics: Theory and applications in speech pathology and therapy (pp. ix-xxiii). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
: Favre, B., Grishman, R., Hillard, D., Hi, H., Hakkani-Tür, D. & Ostendorf, M. (2008). Punctuating speech for information extraction. Spoken Language Technologies. [en línea]. Disponible en: http://www.cs.nyu.edu/hengji/ssie.pdf
: Ferguson, Nicola. (1977). Simultaneous speech, interruptions and dominance. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16 (4), 295-302.
: Fernald, A. & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. Child Development, 64, 637-656.
: Fernald, Anne, Traute Taeschner, Judy Dunn, Mechthild Papousek, Bénédicte de Boysson-Bardies e Ikuko Fukui. 1989. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants, Journal of Child Language, 16: 477-501.
: Fernyhough, Charles y Fradley, Emma. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103-120.
: Fernández-García, M. (1999). Patterns of gender agreement in the speech of second language learners. En J. Gutiérrez-Rexach y F. Martínez-Gil (Eds.), Advances in hispanic linguistics: papers from the 2nd Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 3-15). Cascadilla Press.
: Ferrara, K. y B. Bell (1995), “Sociolinguistic variation and discourse function of constructed dialogue introducers: The case of be+ like”, American Speech, 70:3, pp. 265-290.
: Ferreiro, E. & Zucchermaglio, C. (1996) Children's use of punctuation marks: The case of quoted speech. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B. Burge, & L. Resnick (Eds.) Children's Early Text Construction. Moahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
: Ferré, G. (2010). Timing relationships between speech and co-verbal gestures in spontaneous French. En Proceedings of Language Resources and Evaluation, Workshop on Multimodal Corpora, pp. 86-91.
: Fey, M., Catts, H., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, B. & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of children with Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 1301-1318.
: Feyereisen, P. (1999). Neuropsychology of communicative movements. En L. Messing y R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, Speech and Sign (pp. 3-25). Doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524519.003.0001.
: Finardi, K. & Prebianca, G. 2006. "Working memory capacity and speech production in L2: evidence from a picture description task", in Revista de Estudos da Linguagem. Faculdade de Letras da UFMG 14 (1), pp. 231-260.
: Finardi, K. & Weissheimer, J. 2009. "On the relationship between working memory capacity and L2 speech development", in Signotica 20, pp. 365-390.
: Finardi, K. (2009). Working memory capacity and the acquisition of a syntactic structure in L2 speech (Tesis de doctorado no publicada), Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Pos-Graduação em Letras /Inglés E Literatura Correspondente, Brasil.
: Finardi, K. 2008. "Working memory and speech performance in a picture description task with repetition", in Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos 50, pp. 135-148.
: Flege, J. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. En W. Strange, Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research (pp. 233-277). Nueva York: Baltimore, MD: York Press.
: Flege, J. E. (1988). The production and perception of speech sounds in a foreign languages. En H. Winitz (Ed.), Human communication and its disorders, a Review (pp. 224-401). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
: Flege, J. E. (1992). Speech learning in a second language. En C. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Stoel-Gammon (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, and implications (pp. 565-604). Timonium, MD: York Press.
: Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. En W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 229-273). Timonium, MD: York Press.
: Flege, J. E. (1999). Age of learning and second-language speech. En D. Birdsong (Ed.), New perspectives on the critical period hypothesis for second language acquisition (pp. 101-132). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
: Flege, J. E., & Hillenbrand, J. (1987). Limits on pronunciation accuracy in adult foreign language speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 708-721.
: Flege, J. E., & Munro, M. (1994). The word unit in second language speech perception and production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 381-411.
: Flege, James E. 1995. Second Language Speech Learning: Theory, Findings, and Problems, em Winifred Strange, Speech perception and linguistic experience: issues in cross language research, Timonium, MD: York Press: 233-277.
: Fluharty, G. W., y Ross. H. (1996). Public Speaking and Other Forms of Speech Communication. Nueva York: Barnes & Nobles.
: Fodor, Janet Deán. 2002. "Psycholinguistics cannot escape prosody", en Bernard Bel & Isabelle Marlien (eds.). Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2002. Aix-en-Provence, France, April 2002, pp.83-88.
: Font, C. y Ríos, A. (1991). Compensatory shortening in Spanish spontaneous speech.Proceedings of the ESCA ‘Phonetics and Phonology of Speaking Styles, 16, 1-5.
: Fortkamp, M. B. M. 2000. Working memory capacity and L2 speech production: An exploratory study. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Florianópolis, Brazil: Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina.
: Foster, G. M. (1963). Speech Forms and Perception of Social Distance in a Spanish-Speaking Mexican Village. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 20, 107-122.
: Foulkes, P. , Docherty, G. y Watt, D. (2005). Phonological Variation in Child-Directed Speech. Linguistic Society of America, 81(1), 177-206. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0018
: Fowler, C. A., and D. J. Dekle. 1991. "Listening with eye and hand: Cross-modal contributions to speech perception". Journal Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, pp. 816-828.
: Fowler, C.A., Brown, J., Sabadini, L. y Weihing, J. (2003). Rapid access to speech gestures in perception: Evidence from choice and simple response time tasks.Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 396-413.
: Fraser, H. (2003). Issues in Transcription: Factors affecting the reliability of transcripts as evidence in legal cases.The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 10(2), 203-226.
: Fraser, K. C., Meltzer, J. A. & Rudzicz, F. (2016). Linguistic features identify Alzheimer’s disease in narrative speech. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 49(2), 407-422.
: Freedom Scientific. (2013). Job access with speech JAWS (Version JFW 15.0) [Software lector de pantalla]. St. Petersburg, FL: Freedom Scientific. Recuperado de [51]http://www.freedomscientific.com/ products/fs/JAWS-product-page.asp
: French, J. P. & Harrison, P. (2007). Position statement concerning use of impressionistic likelihood terms in forensic speaker comparison cases. International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 74(1), 137-144.
: Fujisaki, H. (2004). Information, prosody, and modeling. En B. Bel & I. Marlien (Eds.), Proceedings of speech prosody (pp. 1-10). Japon: Nara.
: Fujisaki, Hiroya. (2004). Information, Prosody, and Modeling. Proceedings of Speech Prosody. Japón: Nara.
: Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of child language, 6(3), 423-442. [245]https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900002464
: GOODWIN, P. y W., WENZEL, (1979). “Proverbs and practical reasoning: a study in socio-logic.” En Quarterly journal of speech. (65): 289-302.
: GRICE, H. (1975). Logic and conversation. En P. Cole y J. Morgan (eds.), Speech Acts (Syntax and Semantics, vol. III) (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.
: Gandour, J.T. 2006. The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics: Brain mapping of Chinese speech prosody. Cambridge University Press.
: García Meilán, J., Martínez-Sánchez, F. & Ivanova, O. (2018). Speech production in persons with dementia. En J. García Meilán, F. Martínez-Sánchez & O. Ivanova (Eds.), Current Alzheimer Research, 15(2), 102-103.
: Garnica, O. y Edwards, M. L. (1977). Phonological variation in children’s speech: the trade-off phenomenon. Working Papers y Linguistics, (22), 81-87.
: Gaskell, M. & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 583-607.
: Gass, S. and Varonis, E. M. (1984). The effect of familiarity on the comprehensibility of non-native speech. Language Learning, 34, 65-89.
: Gaudio, R. P. (1994). Sounding gay: pitch properties in the speech of gay and straight men. American Speech, 69(1), 30-57.
: Geis, M. (1995). Speech acts and conversational interaction. Cambridge: CUP.
: Geiser, E., Zaehle, T., Jancke, L., & Meyer, M. (2008). The neural correlate of speech rhythm as evidenced by metrical speech processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 541-552.
: Gerstman, H. L. (1964). A case of aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 29(1), 89-91.
: Gibbs, R. (1986). What makes some indirect speech acts conventional? Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 181-196.
: Gibbs, R. 1991. "Semantic Analyzability in Children's Understanding of Idioms". Journal of Speech and Hearing Research Vol. 34, 613-620.
: Gierut, J. (1999). Syllable onsets: clusters and adjuncts in acquisition. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, (42), 708-726.
: Giles, H. (1971). Patterns of evaluation to R. P., South Welsh and Somerset accented speech. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 280-281.
: Giles, H. (1979). Ethnicity markers in speech. En Scherer, Klaus y Howard Giles. Social markers in Speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Giles, H. y Powesland, P. (1975). Speech style and social evaluation. Londres: Academic Press.
: Giles, H., Mulac, A., Bradac, J., & Johnson, P. (1987). Speech Accommodation Theory: The First Decade and Beyond. En M. McLaughlin (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 10 (pp. 13-48). Newbury Park: Sage.
: Giles, Howard y Peter Powesland. (1975). Speech style and social evaluation. New York, London: Academic Press.
: Gili Fivela, Barbara. 2009. From production to perception and back: An analysis of two pitch accents. In Susanne Fuchs, Héléne Loevenbruck, Daniel Pape and Pascal Perrier (eds.), Some aspects of speech and the brain, 363-405. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.
: Gillam, R.B., & Johnston, J.B. (1992). Spoken and written language relationships in language/learning impaired and normally achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 35(6), 13031315.
: Gold, E. & French, P. (2011). International practices in forensic speaker comparison. International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 18(2), 293-307.
: Goldfield, B. (1993). Noun bias in maternal speech to one-year olds. Journal of Child Language, 20, 85-99.
: Goldman, J. (2011). EasyAlign: An automatic phonetic alignment tool under PRAAT. Interspeech' 11,12th Annual Conference Of The International Speech Communication Association. Recuperado de [94]https://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:18188
: Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. Londres, Nueva York: Academic Press.
: Goldman-Eisler, Frieda. 1972. "Pauses, Clauses, Sentences", en Language and Speech, 15, pp.103-113.
: Goldstein, B. & Iglesias, A. (1996a). Phonological patterns in normally developing Spanish-speaking 3- and 4- years-old of Puerto Rican descent. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 82-90.
: Goldstein, B., Fabiano, L. & Iglesias, A. (2004). Spontaneous and imitated productions in Spanish- speaking children with phonological disorders. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 5-15.
: Goldstein, Brian e Iglesias, Aquiles. 1996. "Phonological Patterns in Normally Developing Spanish — Speaking 3-and 4-years-olds of Puerto Rican Descent", en Language Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 27, pp. 82-90.
: Goldstein, Brian y Swasey Washington, Patricia. 2001. "An initial investigation of phonological pattern in 4-year-old typically developing Spanish-English bilingual children", en Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 32, pp. 153-164.
: Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick y Gail Johnson Ames. 1979. A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ speech with their young children, Child Development, 50: 28-32.
: Goodell, Elizabeth W. y Jacqueline Sachs. 1992. Direct and indirect speech in children’s retold narratives, Discourse Processes, 15: 395-422.
: Goodwin, P. & Wenzel, J. (1979). Proverbs and practical reasoning: A study in socio-Logic. The Quarterly Journal of Speech, 65, 289-302.
: Gordon, J., & Darcy, I. (2016). The development of comprehensible speech in L2 learners: a classroom study on the effects of short-term pronunciation instruction. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 2(1), 56-92. doi: [185]https://doi.org/10.1075/jslp.2.1.03gor
: Gough, P. (1972). One second of reading. En J. Kavanaugh & I. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationships between speech and reading (pp. 331-358). Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.
: Grabe, E. & Low, E. L. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis”. En C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.),Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7(pp. 515-546). Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter.
: Grant, T. (2007). Quantifying evidence for forensic authorship analysis. International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 14(1), 1-25.
: Greenhalgh, K. S. & Strong, C. J. (2001). Literate language features in spoken narratives of children with typical language and children with language impairment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32 (2), 114125.
: Greenslade, K., Plante, E. & Vance, R. (2009). The Diagnostic Accuracy and Construct Validity of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test - Preschool: Second Edition. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 40, (pp. 150-160).
: Grice, H. (1975). Logic and conversation. En J. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Speech acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.
: Grice, H. P. (1998). Further notes on Logic and Conversation. En A. Kasher (Ed.), Pragmatics: Critical Concepts: Vol. 6. Presupposition, implicature and indirect speech (pp. 162-176). London: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.
: Grice, H. Paul. (1975). Logic and conversation. En Peter Cole y Jerry L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics, vol. 3: Speech Acts (pp. 41-58). Nueva York: Academic Press.
: Grinswold, Olga. 2016. Center stage: Direct and indirect reported speech in conversational storytelling, Issues in Applied Linguistics, 20: 73-90. (en línea) Disponible en [152]https://escholarship.org/content/qt19b8197x/qt19b8197x.pdf
: Grunwell, P. (Ed.) (1990). Developmental Speech Disorders. Clinical Issues and Practical Implications. Londres: Churchill Livingstone.
: Grunwell, Pamela. 1985. Phonological Assessment of child speech (PACS). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
: Guará-Tavares, M. d. G. 2008. Pre-task planning, working memory capacity, and L2 speech performance. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Florianópolis, Brazil: Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina.
: Gummersall, Dawn & Strong, Carol. 1999. Assessment of complex sentence production in narrative context. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, pp. 152-164.
: Gumperz, J. (1968). The speech community. International Encyclopedia of Social Sciences (pp. 381-386). London: McMillan.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. & Hofstetter, R. (1994). Syntactic complexity in Spanish narratives: A developmental study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 645-54.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2006). The discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with latino english-speaking children. Journal of speech, language and hearing research, 50(4), 968-981.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. y Hofstetter, R. 1994. "Syntactic complexity in spanish narratives", en Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, pp. 645-654.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, V., Restrepo, A., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2006). Evaluating the Discriminant Accuracy of a Grammatical Measure with Spanish-Speaking Children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 49, (pp. 1209-1223).
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, V.F. e Iglesias, A. (1992). Causal coherence in the oral narratives of Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech and hearing research, 35, 363-372.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, Vera & Hofstetter, Richard. 1994. Syntactic Complexity in Spanish Narratives: A Development Study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 645-654.
: Gutiérrez-Clellen, Vera. 1998. Syntactic skills of Spanish-speaking children with low school achievement. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 29, 207-220.
: Götz, S. (2013). Fluency in Native and Nonnative Speech. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
: Günthner, Susanne. 1999. Polyphony and the `layering of voices´ in reported dialogues: an analysis of the use of prosodic devices in everyday reported speech, Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 5: 685-708.
: Haas, M. H., Chance, S. A., Cram, D. F., Crow, T. J., Luc, A. & Hage, S. (2014). Evidence of pragmatic impairments in speech and proverb interpretation in Schizophrenia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44(4), 469-483.
: Hample, D., y Dallinger, J.M. (1985). Cognitive Editing of Argument Strategies. Presentado en the Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association (Denver, CO, November 7-10, 1985). [141]https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED266505.pdf.
: Hansson, Kristina y Nettelbladt, Ursula. (1995). Grammatical characteristics of Swedish children with SLI. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 589-598.
: Hardcastle, W J. (1994) Assimilation of Alveolar Stops and Nasals in Connected Speech. In J. Windsor Lewis (editor), Studies in General and English Phonetics in Honour of Professor J. D. O'Connor (pp. 49-67). London: Routledge.
: Hardcastle, W. & Beck, J. (Eds.) (2005). A Figure of Speech: A Festschrift for John Laver. Mahwah, Nj: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
: Harris, J., Rogers, W. & Qualls, C. (1998). Written language comprehension in younger and older adults. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 603-617.
: Hartsuiker, R.J. y Kolk, H.H.J. 1998. "Syntactic persistence in Dutch". En Language and Speech, 41 (2), 143-184.
: Haspelmath, M. (2001). Word Classes and Parts of Speech. En International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (pp. 16538-16545). Stanford, CA/Berlin: Pergamon.
: Hayashi, T., and R. Hayashi (1991) Back channel or main channel: A cognitive approach based on floor and speech acts. Pragmatics and Language Learning Monograph Series 2, 119-138.
: Heath, C. (1986). Body Movement and Speech in Medical Interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Herbert, R. K. (1986). Say “Thank you”-or something. American Speech, 61, 76-78.
: Hermes, Dik J. (1998a). Auditory and visual similarity of pitch contours. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 63-72.
: Hess, W. 1983. Pitch Determination of Speech Signals: Algorithms and Devices. Berlin: Springer.
: Hills, E. C. 1925. "Irradiation of Certain Suffixes", en American Speech 1:1, pp. 38-39.
: Hirst, D. J. 2005. "Form and function in the representation of speech prosody". Speech Communication, 46, 334-347.
: Hirst, D. J., Ide, N. & Véronis, J. 1994. "Coding fundamental frequency patterns for multi-lingual synthesis with INTSINT in the MULTEXT project". In Conference Proceedings of the Second ESCA/IEEE Workshop on Speech Synthesis (pp. 77-80).
: Hockett, Charles F. (1960). The Origin of Speech. Scientific American 203, 88-96.
: Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368-1378. [111]https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612
: Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child’s development of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 155-163. [251]https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1968.tb00759.x
: Hoffmann, I., Nemeth, D., Dye, C. D., Pákáski, M., Irinyi, T. & Kálmán, J. (2010). Temporal parameters of spontaneous speech in Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(1), 29-34.
: Hollien, H. & Martin, C.A. (1996). Conducting research on the effects of intoxication on speech. Forensic Linguistics. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 3(1), 107-128.
: Holt, L. L. and A. J. Lotto. 2002. "Behavioral examinations of the neural mechanisms of speech context effects". Hearing Research, 167, pp. 156-169.
: Honglin, Li. (2007). A comparative study of refusal speech acts in Chinese and American English. Canadian Social Science, 3(4), 64-67.
: Howell, S. (1986). Speech acts as one discourse. Man, 21, 79-101.
: However, compared to human judgment, automatic erroneous sound detection is not at all satisfactory (^[53]Strik et al., 2009). We believe that error detection rate can be improved by using error patterns as guidelines for predicting errors in learner’s speech.
: Huber, J. E., & Darling, M. (2011). Effect of Parkinson’s disease on the production of structured and unstructured speaking tasks: Respiratory physiologic and linguistic considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
: Hume, Elizabeth e Keith Johnson. 2001. A model for the interplay of speech perception and phonology, em E. Hume e K. Johnson (eds.),The Role of Speech Perception inPhonology , New York, Academic Press: 3-26
: Hymes, D. (1996). Speech and language: On inequality. En D. Hymes (Ed.), Ethnography, Linguistics, Narrative: Towards an understanding of voice (pp. 25-62). Abingdon, uk: Taylor & Francis.
: In Italian, SVO, VOS, and OVS orders are allowed in conversational speech, and VSO is permitted in written prose. In support of this argument, Vigliocco et al., (1995) present the following examples of the past tense formation in Italian:
: Ingram, David. 1979. Phonological patterns in the speech of young children, em P. Fletcher e M. Garman (eds.), Language acquisition, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 133-149.
: Inoue, Fumio (1996) “Subjective dialect division in Great Britain”. American Speech. 71. 2, 142-161.
: Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 19-40
: Iverson, J. M., Tencer, H. L., Lany, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). The relation between gesture and speech in congenitally blind and sighted language-learners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 105-130.
: Jacobs, G. (1996). Lesbian and gay male language use: a critical review of the literature. American Speech, 71(1), 49-68.
: Jaffe, A. y Walton, S. (2000). The voices people read: orthography and the representation of non-standard speech. Journal of Sociolinguistics 4 (4), 561-587.
: Jakobson, R. (1966). Linguistic types of aphasia. En E. Carterette (Ed.), Brain function III: Speech, language, and communication (pp. 67-91). Berkeley-Los Angeles: University of California Press.
: Jakobson, Roman, Gunnar Fant y Morris Halle. 1952 Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The Distinctive Features and Their Correlates. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
: James, W. (2006). Phonological signatures of tobagonian speech in Trinidad and Tobago. En H. Simmons-McDonald & I. Robertson (Eds.), Exploring the boundaries of Caribbean Creole languages (pp. 50-71). USA: University of the West Indies Press.
: Jamieson, D. G. & Morosan, D. E. (1986). Training non-native speech contrasts in adults: Acquisition of the English /ð/-/θ/ contrast by francophones. Perception, & Psychophysics, 40(4), 205-215.
: Jamieson, D., & Morosan, D. (1989). Training new, nonnative speech contrasts: A comparison of the prototype and perceptual fading techniques. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 88-96. doi: [195]https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084209
: Janse, E., y Newman, R. (2013). Identifying nonwords: Effects of lexical neighborhoods, phonotactic probability, and listener characteristics. Language and Speech, 56 (4), 421-444
: Jiménez, B. (1985). Acquisition of Spanish Consonants in Children Aged 3-5 Years, 7 Months. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 18, (pp. 357-363).
: Johansson, V. (2008). Lexical diversity and lexical density in speech and writing: A developmental perspective, Working Papers, (53), 61-79.
: Johnson, E.K. & Jusczyk, P.W. (2001). Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 548-567.
: Johnstone, Barbara. (1990). Variation in discourse: Midwestern narrative style. American Speech, 65 (3), 195-214.
: Josephs, K. A., Duffy, J. R., Strand, E. A., Machulda, M. M., Senjem, M. L., Master, A. V., Lowe, V. J., Jack Jr, C. R. & Whitwell, J. L. (2012). Characterizing a neurodegenerative syndrome: Primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain, 135(5), 1522-1536.
: Jucker, A. (2009). Speech Act Research between Armchair, Field and Laboratory: The Case of Compliments. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 1611-1635.
: Jurafsky, D. & J. Martin. (2008). An Introduction to Natural Language Processing, Computational Linguistics, and Speech Recognition (2^nd ed.). New York: Prentice Hall.
: Jurafsky, D. & Martin, J. (en preparación). Dependency parsing. Speech and Language Processing (3a ed.) [en línea]. Disponible en: [140]https://web.stanford.edu/~jurafsky/slp3/14.pdf
: Jurafsky, D. & Martin, J. H. 2000. Speech and Language Processing: An introduction to natural language processing, computational linguistics, and speech recognition. Second edition. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
: Jurafsky, Daniel y Martin, James. 2008. Speech and language processing, 2ª edición. USA: Prentice Hall, pp. 683-689.
: Jusczyk, P.W. & Aslin, R.N. (1995). Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1-23.
: KAMHI, A.; LEE, R. & NELSON, L. K. (1985). “Word syllable and sound awareness in language-disordered children”. En Journal of speech and haring disorders, 50, 195- 207.
: Kaderavek, J. N. & Sulzby, E. (2000). Narrative production by children with and without specific language impairment: Oral narratives and emergent Reading. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43(1), 34-49.
: Kamhi, A., Lee, K., & Nelson, L. (1985). Word syllable and sound awareness in language disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 195-207.
: Kashiha, H., & Chan, S. H. (2014) Discourse functions of formulaic sequences in academic speech across two disciplines. GEMA: Online Journal of Language Studies, 14(2), 15-27.
: Kavé, G. & Goral, M. (2018). Word retrieval in connected speech in Alzheimer’s disease: A review with meta-analyses. Aphasiology, 32(1), 4-26.
: Keating, P. (2000). The redfern park speech. En M. Gratton (Ed.), Reconciliation: Essays on Australian reconciliation (pp. 60–64). Melbourne: Black Inc.
: Keith, R. L., & Aronson, A. E. (1975). Singing as therapy for apraxia of speech and aphasia: report of a case. Brain and Language, 2, 483-488.
: Kelly, S. D., Özyürek, A., y Maris, E. (2010). Two sides of the same coin: Speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21(2), 260-267.
: Kendon, A. (1972). Some relationships between body motion and speech. An analysis of an example. En A.W. Siegman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in Dyadic Communication (pp. 177-210). Elsmford, NY: Pergamon Press.
: Kendon, A. (1975). Gesticulation, speech, and the gesture theory of language origins. Sign Language Studies, (9), 349-373.
: Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects for the process of the utterance. En M. R. Key (Ed.), The Relation Between Verbal and Non-Verbal Communication (pp. 207-227). The Hague: Mouton and Co.
: Kennedy, Carol W. y Carl T. Camden. (1983). A new look at interruptions. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 47 (1), 45-58.
: Kent, R. D. (2004). The uniqueness of speech among motor systems. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18(6-8), 495-505.
: Kent, R. D., & Read, C. (2002). The Acoustic Analysis of Speech. San Diego, California: Singular/Thomson Learning.
: Kerswill, P. (1994). Dialects converging: Rural speech in urban Norway. Oxford University Press.
: Key words: French-Spanish legal-institutional speech, French-Spanish legal-institutional translation, teaching in Translation & Interpretation, French-Spanish legal-institutional terminology, translation strategies.
: Keyser, Samuel Jay e Kenneth Noble Stevens. 2006. Enhancement and overlap in the speech chain, Language: 82: 33-63.
: Keywords: Acoustics phonetics, vowels, F1 & F2, urban speech, rural speech.
: Kireva, E. y Gabriel, C. (2015). Rhythmic Properties of a Contact Variety: Comparing Read and Semi-Spontaneous Speech in Argentinean Porteño Spanish. En M. Avanzi, E. Delais-Roussarie y S. Herment (eds.), Prosody and Languages in Contact (pp. 149-168). Berlín: Springer-Verlag.
: Kirk, C. (2008). Substitution errors in the production of word-initial and word-final consonant clusters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1-14.
: Kohnen, T. (2007). Text types and the methodology of diachronic speech act analysis. En S. M. Fitzmaurice & I. Taavitsainen (Eds.), Methods in historical pragmatics (pp. 139-166). Berlín-Nueva York: Mouton de Gruyter.
: Koike, A. 1989. Pragmatic Competence and adult Second language Acquisition: Speech Acts in Interlenguage, The Modern Language Journal, 73, 3: 279-289.
: Kormos, J. (2006). Speech production and second language acquisition. Magwag, Nueva Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum .
: Kotz, S. A., & Schmidt-Kassow, M. (2015). Basal ganglia contribution to rule expectancy and temporal predictability in speech. Cortex , 68, 48-60.
: Kotz, S. A., & Schwartze, M. (2010). Cortical speech processing unplugged: a timely subcortico- cortical framework. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(9), 392-399.
: Kowal, Sabine, Richard Wiese, Daniel O’Connell. 1983. The use of time in storytelling, Language and Speech, 26, 4: 377-392.
: Kredens, K. & Gorálewska-Lach, G. (1998). Language as sole incriminating evidence: The Augustynek case. Forensic Linguistics. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 5(2), 193-202.
: Krestel, R. (2007) Automatic analysis and reasoning on reported speech in newspaper articles. Tesis de Magister Universität Karlsruhe. Karlsruhe. Alemania. Disponible en [58]http://www.semanticsoftware.info/system/files/believer.pdf.
: Kuhl, P. (2010a). Cracking the Speech Code: Language and the Infant Brain. The twelveth Benjamin and Anne A. Pinkel Lecture on mind/brain paradigms. Pensilvania: University of Pennsylvania.
: Kuhl, P. K. & Iverson, P. (1995). Linguistic experience and the ‘Perceptual Magnet Effect.’ In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-language Research (pp. 121-154). Baltimore, MD: York Press.
: Kvavik, K. H. (1986), “Characteristics of direct and reported speech prosody: Evidence from Spanish”, en F. Coulmas (ed.), Direct and Indirect Speech, La Haya, Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 333-360.
: Köder, Franziska y Emar Maier. 2016. Children mix direct and indirect speech: evidence from pronoun comprehension, Journal of Child Language , 43,4: 843-866.
: Künzel, H. J. (1997). Some general phonetic and forensic aspects of speaking tempo. Forensic Linguistics. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 4(1), 48-83.
: LEVY Y. (1999). “Early metalinguistic competence: speech monitoring and repair behavior”. En Developmental psychology, 35, 3, 822-834.
: Ladd, D. Robert. (1981). On Intonational Universals. Myers, T. et al. (Eds): The Cognitive Representation of Speech (389-397). Amsterdam, Holanda: North Holland Publishing.
: Langeveld-Cambier, T. (2007). Current methods in forensic speaker identification: Results of a collaborative exercise. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 14(2), 223-243.
: Larson, M. L. (1977). The function of reported speech in discourse. Tesis doctoral, University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, Estados Unidos.
: Law, James, Boyle, James, Harris, Frances, Harkness, Avril y Nye, Chad. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(2), 165-188.
: Laws, J.V. & C. Ryder (2014). Getting the Measure of Derivational Morphology in Adult Speech: A Corpus Analysis Using Morpho Quantics. Language Studies Working Papers, 6, 3-17.
: Lazaraton, A. (2004). Gesture and speech in the vocabulary explanations of one ESL teacher: A microanalytic inquiry. Language Learning, 54(1), 79-117.
: Le Guen, O. (2011). Speech and gesture in spatial language and cognition among the Yucatec Mayas. Cognitive Science, 35(5), 905-938. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01183.x.
: Leahy, Margareth. 2004. Therapy Talk: Analyzing Therapeutic Discourse, Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 1: 70-81.
: Lee, A. H., & Lyster, R. (2016). The effects of corrective feedback on instructed L2 speech perception. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(1), 1-30.
: Lee, C. S., & Todd, N. P. M. (2004). Towards an auditory account of speech rhythm: application of a model of the auditory ‘primal sketch’to two multi-language corpora. Cognition, 93(3), 225- 254.
: Leeuwen, Theo van. 1999. Speech, Music, Sound, London, Macmillan.
: Lehiste, I. 1975. "The phonetic structure of paragraphs", en A. Cohen y S.G. Nooteboom (eds.), Structure and process in speech perception, pp.195-203. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Sprenger-Verlag.
: Leonard, L. & Dispaldro, M. (2013). The effects of production demands on grammatical weaknesses in specific language impairment: The case of clitic pronouns in italian. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1272-1286.
: Leonard, L., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C., Francis, D., Tombling, J. & Kail, R. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 50, 408-428. Doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/029).
: Leonard, T. & Cummins, F. (2010). The temporal relation between beat gestures and speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1295-1309.
: Leskiv, Alina. 2009. The literary phenomenon of free indirect speech, Studia Anglica Rsoviensia, 6: 51-58.
: Levelt, W. J. L., Roelofs, A. & Meyer, A.S. 1999. "A theory of lexical access in speech production", en Behavioural and Brain Sciences 22, 1-75.
: Levitt, A.G. & A.F. Healy. 1985. "The roles of phoneme frequency, similarity, and availability in the experimental elicitation of speech errors". Journal of Memory and Language 24: 717-733.
: Levy, Y. (1999). Early metalinguistic competence: Speech monitoring and repair behavior. Developmental Psychology, 3, 822-834.
: Liberman, A. & Mattingly, I. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1-36.
: Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Schanweiler, D. P. y Studdert-Kennedy, M. 1967. "Perception of Speech Code", en Psychological Review 74, pp. 431-461.
: Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(5), 358-368.
: Liberman, Mark y Sag, I. (1975). Intonational Disambiguation of Indirect Speech Acts. CLS, 11, 487-497.
: Lieberman, P. & Blumstein, S. E. 1988. Speech Physiology Speech Perception and Acoustic Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Lieberman, P. 1991. Uniquely Human: The evolution of speech, thought and selfless behaviour . Cambridge , Mass. : Harvard University Press.
: Liles, B. (1987). Episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children with and without language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 30, 185-196.
: Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D.D. y Purcell, S. L. (1995). Measurement of Narrative Discourse Ability in Children with Language Disorders, Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(2), 415-425.
: Lindblom, B., & Maddieson, I. (1988). Phonetic universals in consonant systems. En L. M. Hyman & C. N. Li (eds.), Language, speech and mind (pp. 62-78). London: Routledge.
: Linell, Per. (1982). The concept of phonological form and the activities of speech production and speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 10, 37-42.
: Lisker, L. & A. Abramson. (1967). Voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech 10(1),1-28. [140]https://doi.org/10.1177/002383096701000101
: Liu, Ch. y Jin, S-H. 2015. Auditory detection of non-speech and speech stimuli in noise: Effects of listeners' native language background. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138, 2782-2790.
: Loehr, D. (2007). Aspects of rhythm in gesture and speech. Gesture, 7, 179-214.
: Logan, J. S., & Pruitt, J. S. (1995). Methodological issues in training listeners to perceive non-native phonemes. En W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: issues in cross-language research (pp. 351-378). Baltimore: York Press.
: Lotfollahi, B., & Eslami-Rasekh, A. (2011). Speech act of condolence in Persian and English: A cross-cultural study. Studies in Literature and Language, 13(3), 139-145.
: Lotto, A.J. and K.R. Kluender. 1998. "General contrast effects in speech perception: Effect of preceding liquid on stop consonant identificatión". Perception and Psychophysics, 60, pp. 602.
: Low, E. L., & Grabe, E. (1999). A Contrastive study of Prosody and lexical stress placement in Singapore English and British English. Language and Speech, 42(1), 39-56.
: Luce, P. & McLennan, C. (2005). Spoken word recognition. En D. Pisoni & R. Remez (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 591-609). Malden, Ma.: Blackwell.
: Lucy, J. (1993). Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: López-de-Ipiña, K., Alonso, J. B., Travieso, C. M., Solé-Casals, J., Egiraun, H., Faundez-Zanuy, M. & Lizardui, U. M. D. (2013). On the selection of non-invasive methods based on speech analysis oriented to automatic Alzheimer disease diagnosis. Sensors, 13(5), 6730-6745.
: MacNeilage, P.F. & Davis, B.L. (1990). Acquisition of speech production: Achievement of segmental independence. Hardcastle, W.I. & Marchal, A. (Eds.) Speech Production and Speech Modeling (pp. 55-68). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
: Maclagan, M., Davis, B. & Lunsford, R. (2008). Fixed expressions, extenders and metonymy in the speech of people with Alzheimer’s disease. En S. Granger & F. Meunier (Eds.), Phraseology: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 175-187). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
: Maillart, Christelle; Schelstraete, Marie Anne y Hupet, Michelle. 2004. "Phonological representations in children with SLI: a study of French", en Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 47 (1), pp. 187-199.
: Manes, J. y Wolfson, N. (1980). The compliment formula. En F. Coulmas (Ed.), Conversational Routine: Explorations in Standardized Communication Situations and Prepatterned Speech (pp. 116-132). La Haya, Países Bajos: Mouton Publishers.
: Marcus, G.F. y Fisher, S.E. 2003. "FOXP2 in focus: what can genes tell us about speech and language? Trends", en Cognitive Science 7, pp. 257-262.
: Marinellie, S. y Kneile, L. (2012). Acquiring knowledge of derived nominals and derived adjectives in context. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 43(1), 53-65. [120]https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0053).
: Marslen-Wilson, W. & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical accessduring word recognition in continuos speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 29-63.
: Marslen-Wilson, W. D. 1990. "Activation, competition and frequency in lexical access". En G. T. Altmann (Ed.). Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
: Martínez-Flor, A. (2005). A theoretical review of the speech act of suggesting: Towards a taxonomy for its use in FLT. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses, 18, 167-187.
: Martínez-Sánchez, F., Meilán, J. J. G., Vera-Ferrandiz, J. A., Carro, J., Pujante-Valverde, I., Ivanova, O. & Carcavilla, N. (2017). Speech rhythm alterations in Spanish-speaking individuals with Alzheimer’s disease. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 24(4), 418-434.
: Massaro, D.W. 1987. Speech Perception by Ear and by Eye: A Paradigm of Psychological Inquiry. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum.
: Massaro, T. M. (2011). Equality and freedom of expression: The hate speech dilemma. William and Mary Law Review, 32(2), 211-265.
: Mattys, S. L., & Samuel, A. G. (1997). How lexical stress affects speech segmentation and interactivity: Evidence from the migration paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language , 36(1), 87-116.
: McCauley, R. & Swisher, L. (1984). Psychometric review of language and articulation tests for preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 49, (pp. 34-42).
: McClelland, J. & Elman, J. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86.
: McKenna, P. J. y Oh, T. (2005). Schizophrenic speech: making sense of bathroots and ponds that fall in doorway. Londres: Cambridge University Press.
: McLeod, Sharynne y Harrison, Linda. (2009). Epidemiology of speech and language impairment in a nationally representative sample of 4-to 5-year-old children. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research. 52:113-129.
: McNeil, D. (2002). Triangulating the growth point-arriving at consciousness. En L. S. Messing & R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 77-92). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: McNeill, D. (2000c). Catchments and contexts: non-modular factors in speech and gesture production. En D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 312-328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Medellín (Castañeda Naranjo, 2005, pp. 76-77), as these are not restricted to the speech of urban areas, but are present in certain rural areas of the Colombian territory.
: Meilijson, S. R.; Kasher, A., y Elizur, A. (2004). 'Language performance in chronic schizophrenia: a pragmatic approach'. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47(3): 695-713.
: Meilán, J. J., Martinez-Sánchez, F., Carro, J., López, D. E., Millian-Morell, L. & Arana, J. M. (2014). Speech in Alzheimer’s disease: Can temporal and acoustic parameters discriminate dementia? Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 37(5-6), 327-334.
: Merrell, A. & Plante, E. (1997). Norm-referenced test interpretation in the diagnostic process. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 28, (pp. 50-58).
: Mertens, P 2004. "The Prosogram: Semi-Automatic Transcription of Prosody based on a Tonal Perception Model". In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2004 (pp. 549-552). Consultado el 24 de junio de 2007 en[140] http://bach.arts.kuleuven.be/pmertens/papers/sp2004.pdf
: Miatto, V., Hamann, S., & Boersma, P. (2019). Self-reported L2 input predicts phonetic variation in the adaptation of English final consonants into Italian. Canberra: Australasian Speech Sci ence and Technology Association Inc.
: Miller C. R. (1984). Genre as social action. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 70(2), 151-167.
: Miller, K. (2013). Acquisition of variable rules: /s/-lenition in the speech of Chilean Spanish-speaking children y their caregivers. Language, Variation and Change, (25), 311-340. https://doi.org/10.1017/s095439451300015x
: Miller, Paul (1997). "The Effect of Communication Mode on the Development of Phonemic Awareness in Prelingually Deaf Students", Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40-5, 1151-1163.
: Minor, E. E., & Loos, E. E. (1963). The structure and contexts of Witoto predicates in narrative speech. En B. F. Elson (Ed.), Studies in Peruvian Indian languages 1 (pp. 37-66). Norman: Summer Institute of Linguistics of the University of Oklahoma.
: Miolo, G., Chapman, R. & Sindberg, H. (2005). Sentence comprehension in adolescents with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48(1), 172-188.
: Moats, L. C. (2010). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers (2nd ed.). Brookes Publishing.
: Moeschler, J. (2001). Speech act theory and the analysis of conversation. En D. Vanderverken & S. Kubo (Eds.), Essays in speech act theory (pp. 239-262). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
: Mokbel, Chafic, Laurent Mauuary, Lamia Karray, Denis Jouvet, Jean Monné, Jacques Simonin e Katarina Bartkova. 1997. Towards improving ASR robustness for PSN and GSM telephone applications, Speech Communication, 23, 1-2: 141-159.
: Montero, Ignacio y De Dios, María Jesús. (2006). Vygotsky was right: An experimental approach to the study of the relationship between private speech and task performance. Estudios de Psicología, 27, 175-189.
: Morady Moghaddam, M. (2012). Discourse structures of condolence speech act. Journal of English Language Teaching and Learning, 10, 105-125.
: Moraes, João Antônio de. 2008. The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: analysis by synthesis, em P. Barbosa, S. Madureira e C. Reis (eds.), Speech Prosody 2008: fourth conference on speech prosody, Campinas, RG/CNPq: 389-397.
: Morgan, Jerry. 1978. "Two types of convention in indirect speech acts". En Peter Cole (ed.) Syntax and semantics. Vol 9: Pragmatics. Nueva York: Academic Press.
: Morsella, E. & Krauss, R. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 117(2), 411 -424.
: Mozziconacci, Sylvie. (1998). Speech Variability and Emotion: Production and Perception. Eindhoven: Proefschrift.
: Mueller, K. D., Hermann, B., Mecollari, J. & Turkstra, L. S. (2018). Connected speech and language in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A review of picture description tasks. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 40(9), 917-939.
: Mullennix, John.W., and D. B. Pisoni. 1990. "Stimulus variability and processing dependencies in speech perception". Perception and Psychophysics, 47, pp. 379-390.
: Munro, M. and Derwing, T. (1999). Foreign accent, comprehensibility, and intelligibility in the speech of second language learners. Language Learning, 49, 285-310.
: Munson, B. (2007). The acoustic correlates of perceived masculinity, perceived femininity, and perceived sexual orientation. Language and Speech, 50(1), 125-142.
: Murphy, B. &. Neu, J. (1996). My grade's too low: the speech act set of complaining. In S. M. Gass & J. Neu (Eds.), Speech Acts Across Cultures: Challenges to Communication in Second Language, 191-216, Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
: Muysken, Pieter. Bilingual speech: A typology of code-mixing. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 2000.
: Myers, G. (1992). "In this text we report..." Speech acts and scientific facts. Journal of Pragmatics, 17, 295-313.
: Nakamura, K. (2003). Global Literacy as a New Paradigm for EIL Education: Integrating Global Issues into EIL Speech Communication Class. Language and culture: The Journal of the Institute for Language and Culture, 7, 1-24. doi: 10.14990/00000378.
: Nastri, J., Peña, J. y Hancock, J.T. (2006). The construction of away messages: A speech act analysis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11 (4), 7-16.
: National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. (2016). Speech and Language Disorders in Children : Implications for the Social Security Administration's Supplemental Security Income Program. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/21872.
: Newman, R. S., & Hussain, I. (2006). Changes in preference for infant-directed speech in low and moderate noise by 5- to 13-month-olds. Infancy, 10(1), 61-76. [282]https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1001_4
: Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Dow, K. A. (2006). Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: A retrospective analysis. Developmental psychology, 42(4), 643.
: Newman, Rochelle; James Sawusch and Paul Luce. 2000. "Underspecification and phoneme frequency in speech perception", Papers in Laboratory Phonology V: Acquisition and the Lexicon. Eds. M. B. Broe and J. Pierrehumbert. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 299-312.
: Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Albert, M. L. & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1985). Empty speech in Alzheimer’s disease and fluent aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 28(3), 405-410.
: Nilsson, M., Soli, S., y Sullivan, J. 1994. Development of the Hearing in Noise Test for the measurement of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(2),1085-1099.
: Nippold, M. 1995. "Language norms in school-age children and adolescents: An introduction", en Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 26, pp. 307-308. October.
: Nippold, M., Hesketh, L., Duthie, J. y Mansfield, T 2005. "Conversational versus expository discourse: A study syntactic development in children, adolescents and adults", en Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 48, pp. 1048-1064.
: Nippold, M., Ward-Lonergan, J., & Fanning, J. (2005). Persuasive writing in children, adolescents, and adults. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 125. 138.
: Nippold, Marilyn, Hesketh, Linda, Duthie, Jill & Mansfield, Tracy. 2005. Conversational versus expository discourse: A study syntactic development in children, adolescents and adults, en Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, pp. 1048-1064.
: Nishimura S. (2005). Languaje, violence and irrevocability: speech acts in tess of the dúrbervilles. En: Studies in the Novel volumen 37 N°2. pp. 208-222.
: Nishimura, M., & Yoon, K. (1998). Head Directionality and Intrasentential Code-switching: A Study of Japanese Canadians and Korean Americans' Bilingual Speech. Japanese/Korean Linguistics 8, Stanford: CSLI Publications, 121-130.
: Nobe, S. (1996). Representational gestures, cognitive rhythms, and acoustic aspects of speech: a network/threshold model of gesture production. Tesis doctoral, Universidad de Chicago, Chicago, USA.
: Noland Grand C. (2009). Janes Austen's Speech Acts and Languaged Based Societies. (Baltimore). En: SEL Studies in English Literature 1500-1900. pp. 863-878.
: Nooteboom, S. (1997), “Prosody of speech: Melody and rhythm”, en W. J. Hardcastle y J. Laver (eds.), The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences, Oxford, Blackwell, pp. 640-673.
: Nordqvist, Asa. 2001. The use of direct and indirect speech by 1 ½ - to 4-year-olds, Psychology of Language and Communication, 5: 57-66. (en línea) Disponible en [166]http://www.plc.psychologia.pl/plc/plc/contents/fulltext/05-1_4.pdf
: Norrick, N. (1980). "The speech act of complimenting". In: E. Hovdhaugen (Ed.). The Nordic language and modern linguistics, 296-304. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.
: Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A conectionism model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189-234.
: Norris, D., McQueen, J. & Cutler, A. (2000). Merging information in speech recognition: Feedback is never necessary. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 23, 299-335.
: OLSON, D. R. (1996). Towards a Psychology of Literacy: On the Relations Between Speech and Writing. En Cognition, 60, 83-104.
: Ogbu, J. (1999). Beyond language: Ebonics, proper English, and identity in a black American speech community. American Educational Research Journal, 36(2), summer/ 99. doi: [249]https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312036002147
: Ohala, J. J. (1993). Coarticulation and phonology. Language and Speech, 3(5(2-3), 155-170.
: Ohala, John J. 1993. Sound change as nature's speech perception experiment,Speech Communication, 13: 155-161.
: Olaszi, Péter. 2000. Analysis of Written and Spoken Form of Hungarian Numbers for TTS Applications, International Journal of Speech Technology, 3, 3: 177-186.
: Oller, D. K., Wieman, L .A., Doyle, W. J. & Ross, C. (1976). Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language, 3, 1-11.
: Oller, D.K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
: Olshtain and Weinbach (1993, p. 108) asserted "in the speech act of complaining, the speaker (S) expresses displeasure or annoyance -censure- as a reaction to a past or going action, the consequences of which are perceived by S as affecting her unfavorably."
: Olshtain, E. & Weinbach, L. (1993). Interlanguage features of the speech act of complaining. In G. Kasper & S. Blum-Kulka (Eds.), Interlanguage Pragmatics, 108-122, New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: Olshtain, E., & Blum-Kulka, S. (1985). Degree of approximation: Nonnative reactions to native speech act behavior. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 303-325). New York: Newbury House.
: Olshtain, E., & Cohen, A. (1991). Teaching speech act behavior to nonnative speakers. In M. Celce-Murcia (Ed.), Teaching English as a second or foreign language (pp. 154-165). Boston: Heinle & Heinle Publishers.
: Olson, D. (1977). From Utterance to Text: The Bias of Language in Speech and Writing, Harvard Educational Review, 47 (3), 257-281.
: Olson, D. J. (2014). Phonetics and technology in the classroom: A practical approach to using speech analysis software in second-language pronunciation instruction. Hispania, 97(1), 47-68.
: Orange, J. B., Lubinski, R. B. & Higginbotham, D. J. (1996). Conversational repair by individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(4), 881-895.
: Orozco-Arroyave, J., Vásquez-Correa, J., Vargas-Bonilla, J., Arora, R., Dehak, N., Nidadavolu, P., y Nöth, E. (2018). NeuroSpeech: An open-source software for Parkinson’s speech analysis. Digital Signal Processing, 77, 207-221. doi: 10.1016/j.dsp.2017.07.004.
: Orton, S. (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children. Nueva York: Norton.
: Otheguy, R., Zentella, A. C., & Livert, D. (2007). Language and Dialect Contact in Spanish in New York: Toward the Formation of a Speech Community. Language, 83(4), 770-802. doi: [185]https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2008.0019
: O’Brien, Marion y Keith J. Nagle. 1987. Parents’ speech to toddlers: The effect of play context, Journal of Child Language , 14: 269-279.
: Palacios Martínez, I. M. (2013), “Zero quoting in the speech of British and Spanish teenagers: A contrastive corpus-based study”, Discourse Studies, 15:4, pp. 439-462.
: Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1-15.
: Paradis, Johanne y Martha Crago. 2000. Tense and temporality: A comparison between children learning a second language and children with SLI, Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43: 834-847.
: Paul, R. & Smith, R. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds eight normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 592-598.
: Paulino, A. & Sierra, G. (2017). Applying the rhetorical structure theory in alzheimer patients’ speech. Ponencia presentada en el 6th Workshop on Recent Advances in RST and Related Formalisms. Santiago de Compostela, España.
: Paulino, A., Sierra, G., Hernández-Domínguez, L., da Cunha, I. & Bel-Enguix, G. (2018). Rhetorical relations in the speech of Alzheimer’s patients and healthy elderly subjects: An approach from the RST. Computación y Sistemas, 22(3), 895-905.
: Pegg, J. E., Werker, J. F., & McLeod, P. J. (1992). Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15(3), 325-345. [290]https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(92)80003-D
: Penfield, W. and Roberts L. (1959). Speech and Brain Mechanisms. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
: Pennington, Bruce F. y Bishop, Dorothy. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283-306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548.
: Peutz, V.m.A. 1971. Articulation loss of consonants as a criterion for speech transmission in a room. Journal of Audio Engineering Society, 19(11), 23-27.
: Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44(1), 182-185. [292]https://doi.org/10.2307/1127699
: Pisoni, D. B. 1996. "Some thoughts on Anormalization in Speech Perception". En K. Johnson y J. W. Mullennix (Eds.). Talker variability in speech processing. San Diego: Academic Press.
: Pitt, M. A., & Samuel, A. G. (1990). The use of rhythm in attending to speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance , 16(3), 564.
: Plante, E. & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of Preschool Language Tests: A Data-Based Approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 25, (pp. 15-24).
: Poole, M. L., Brodtmann, A., Darby, D. & Vogel, A. P. (2017). Motor Speech Phenotypes of Frontotemporal Dementia, Primary Progressive Aphasia, and Progressive Apraxia of Speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(4), 897-911.
: Prebianca, G. 2010. Working memory capacity and foreign language speech production: a look at lexical access processes and level of proficiency.Saarbrücken, Germany: Lambert Academic Publishing, 234 pp.
: Prebianca, G. V. (2009). Communication strategies and proficiency levels in L2 speech production: A systematic relationship. Revista de Estudos da Linguagem, 17(1), 7-50.
: Price, J. Roberts, J. y Jackson, S. (2006). Structural Development of the Fictional Narratives of African American Preschoolers. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 37(3), 178-187.
: Prieto, P. (2006). The relevance of metrical information in early prosodic word acquisition: A comparison of Catalan and Spanish. Language and Speech, 49, 231-259.
: Prutting, C. & Kirchner, D. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of speech and hearing disorders , 52(2), 105-119. https://doi. org/10.1044/jshd.5202.105
: Préfontaine, Y. (2013). Perceptions of French fluency in second language speech production. Canadian Modern Language Review, 69, 324-348.
: Pérez Hernández, L. & Ruiz de Mendoza, F. (2002). Grounding, semantic motivation, and conceptual interaction in indirect directive speech acts. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 259-284.
: Radford, L (2003). Gestures, Speech, and the Sprouting of Signs: A Semiotic-Cultural Approach to Students' Types of Generalization. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 5(1), 37-70.
: Rahim, Mazin, Giuseppe Riccardi, Lawrence Saul, Jerry Wright, Bruce Buntschuh e Allen Gorin. 2001. Robust numeric recognition in spoken language dialogue, Speech Communication , 34, 1-2: 195-212.
: Rajaganapathy, S., Aravind, B., Keerthana, B., y Sivagami, M. (2015). Conversation of sign language to speech with human gestures. Procedia Computer Science, 50, 10-15. [144]https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2015.04.004
: Ramus, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal”.Cognition, 73(3), 265-292.
: Ramírez, A. (1988). Analyzing speech acts. En J. Green & J. O. Harker (eds.), Multiple perspective analysis of classroom discourse (pp. 135-163). Norwood (Nj): Ablex Publishing Corporation.
: Ramírez, Juan Daniel. (1992). The functional differentiation of social and private speech: A dialogic approach. En R. M. Díaz y L. E. Berk (eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 199-214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
: Rasmussen, Mary Lou y Valerie Hardwood (2003). Performativity, Youth and Injurious Speech. Teaching Education 14: 25-36.
: Restrepo, A. (1998). Identifiers of Predominantly Spanish-Speaking Children With Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, (pp. 1398-1411).
: Rice, M. & Blossom, M. (2013). What do children with Specific Language Impairment do with multiple forms of DO? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(1), 222-235.
: Rickford, J. R., Wasow, T., Zwicky, A., & Buchstaller, I. (2007). Intensive and quotative all: Something old, something new. American Speech, 82(1), 3-31. [117]https://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-2007-001
: Ringrose, C. (2015). Pitch Change in Dog-Directed Speech. Lifespans and Styles, 1, 28-35. doi: 10.2218/ls.v1i0.2015.1181.
: Rintell, E. (1984). But How Did You Feel about That? The Learner's Perception of Emotion in Speech. Applied Linguistics 5(3), 255-264.
: Ripich, D. N. & Terrell, B. Y. (1988). Patterns of discourse cohesion and coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53(1), 8-15.
: Rochon, E., Leonard, C. & Goral, M. (2018). Speech and language production in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology, 32, 1-3.
: Roelofs, A. (1997). The WEAVER model of Word-form encoding in speech production. Cognition, 64, 249-284.
: Roelofs, A. (2005). Spoken word planning, comprehending, and self-monitoring: Evaluation of WEAVER++. En R. Hartsuiker, R. Bastiaanse, A. Postma, y F. Wijnen (Eds.), Phonological encoding and monitoring in normal and pathological speech (pp. 42-63). New York: Psychology Press.
: Romaine, S. y D. Lange (1991), “The use of like as a marker of reported speech and thought: A case of grammaticalization in progress”, American Speech, 66:3, pp. 227-279.
: Romano, A., & Mairano, P. (2010). Speech rhythm measuring and modelling: pointing out multi-layer and multi-parameter assessments. En M. Russo, Prosodic Universals. Comparative Studies in Rhythmic Modeling and Rhythmic Typology (pp. 79-116). Napoli: Aracne.
: Romero-Fresco, P. (2011). Subtitling Through Speech Recognition: Respeaking Manchester: St. Jerome Publishing.
: Rondon-Pari, G. (2011). Comparative analysis of classroom speech in upper level Spanish college courses: A social constructivist view. Contemporary Issues in Education Research, 4(12), pp. 1-18.
: Rosaldo, M. (1982). The Things We Do with Words: Ilongot Speech Acts and Speech Act Theory in Philosophy. Language in Society, 11(2), 203-37.
: Rose, P. & Morrison, G. (2009). A response to the UK position statement on forensic speaker comparison. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 16, 139-193.
: Roustan, B. & Dohen, M. (2010). Co-production of contrastive prosodic focus and manual gestures: Temporal coordination and effects on the acoustic and articulatory correlates of focus. En Proceeding of the Speech Prosody, Chicago.
: Rowe, Meredith L. 2008. Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development, and child vocabulary skill, Journal of Child Language , 35: 185-205.
: SEARLE, J., (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Sachs, J., Brown, R. & Salerno, R.A. (1976). Adults' speech to children. In W. von Raffler-Engel & Y. LeBrun (eds.), Baby talk and infant speech(special issue). Neurolinguistics, 5, 240-245.
: Sadock, J. (1994) "Toward a grammatically realistic typology of speech acts".393- 406. En L. Savas (edit.) Foundations of speech act theory. London: Routledge.
: Sajjadi, S. A., Patterson, K., Tomek, M. & Nestor, P. J. (2012). Abnormalities of connected speech in semantic dementia vs Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology, 26(6), 847-866.
: Salverda, A., Dahan, D. y McQueen, J. (2003). The role of the prosodic boundaries in the resolution of lexical embedding in speech comprehension. Cognition, (90), 51 - 89.
: Samper Padilla, J. A. & Samper Hernández, M. (2020). The weakening of intervocalic /d/ in the Spanish of Las Palmas de Gran Canaria: Comparison with other Speech Communities, Spanish in Context, 17(2), 221-246.
: San Martín, Conchi, Boada, Humbert y Feigenbaum, Peter. (2011). Private and inner speech and the regulation of social speech communication. Cognitive Development, 26 (3), 214-229.
: Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.
: Saville-Troike, Muriel. (1988). Private speech: Evidence for second language learning strategies during the 'silent' period. Journal of Child Language, 15, 567-590.
: Schachter, P. (1985). Parts of Speech Systems. En T. Shopen (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description (vol. 1, pp. 3-61). Cambridge, London, New York, New Rochelle, Melbourne, Sydney: Cambridge University Press.
: Schilling-Estes, N. (1998). Investigating 'self-conscious' speech: The performance register in Ocracoke English. Language in Society, 27, 53-83.
: Schirmer, A. (2004). Timing speech: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2), 269-287.
: Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2009). Attention and perceptual regularity in speech. NeuroReport, 20(18), 1643-1647.
: Schouten, M. E. H. (ed.). 1987. The psychophysics of speech perception. Dordrecht: M. Nijhoff Publ.
: Scott, C. & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 324-339.
: Scott, R. L. (1967). On viewing rhetoric as epistemic. Central States Speech Journal, 18(1), 9-17. [348]http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10510976709362856
: Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak. A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech . Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
: Searle, J. (1969). Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J. (1975). Indirect speech acts. En P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics, 3: Speech Acts (pp. 59-82). Nueva York: Academic Press.
: Searle, J. (1979) A Taxonomy of Illocutionary Acts. En J. Searle, Expression and Meaning: Studies in the Theory of Speech Acts (pp.1-29). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J. (1979). Expression and meaning. Studies in the Theory of Speech Acts. Cambridge: CUP.
: Searle, J. (1979). Speech Acts. New York: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Inglaterra: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J.R. (1969). Speech Acts. New York: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, J.R. (1971). What is a speech act. In J.R. Searle (Ed.), The philosophy of language (pp. 39-54). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
: Searle, J.R., 1969. Speech Acts. London & New York: CUP.
: Searle, John (1975), “Indirect Speech Acts”, en Peter Cole y Jerry L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts, Nueva York, Academic Press, pp. 59-82.
: Searle, John. 1969. Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Searle, John. 1975. "Indirect Speech Acts". En Peter Cole y Jerry L. Morgan (eds.) Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3: Speech acts. Nueva York: Academic Press.
: Sebastián‐Gallés, N. (2005). Cross‐Language Speech Perception. En Pisoni, & D. B. R. E. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 546-566). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing .
: Serra, Carolina R. 2016. A interface prosódia-sintaxe e o fraseamento prosódico no português do Brasil, Joss Journal of Speech Science, 5: 47-86.
: Serratrice, Ludovica, Anne Hesketh y Rachel Ashworth 2015. The use of reported speech in children’s narratives: A priming study. First Language, 35: 68-87. (en línea) Disponible en [178]https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723715569552
: Sherzer, J. (1974). Namakke, sunmakke, dormakke: Three types of Cuna speech event. En R. Bauman y J. Sherzer (Eds.), Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking (pp. 263-82). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .
: Shillcock, R.C.; E.G. Bard y F. Spensley. 1988. Some prosodic effects on human word recognition in continuous speech. Proceedings of SPEECH '88 (Seventh Symposium of the Federation of Acoustic Societies of Europe), Edinburgh (pp. 819-826).
: Shinzato, R. (2004). Some observations concerning mental verbs and speech act verbs. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(5), 861-882. doi: 10.1016/S0378-2166(03)00002-X.
: Shockey, L. y Bond, Z. (1980). Phonological processes in speech addressed to children. Phonetica, 37(4), 267-274.
: Shriberg, E., Stolcke, A., Hakkani-Tür, D. & Tür, G. (2000). Prosody-based automatic segmentation of speech into sentences and topics. Speech Communication, 32(1-2), 127-154.
: Shriberg, Lawrence D., Tomblin, J. Bruce y McSweeny, Jane L. (1999). Prevalence of speech delay in 6-yearold children and comorbidity with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1461-1481. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4206.1461.
: Silverstein, M. (1993). Metapragmatic discourse and metapragmatic function. En J. Lucy (Ed.), Reflexive Language: Reported Speech and metapragmatic (pp. 33-58). Cambridge, UK: University Press.
: Simonet, M., J.I. Hualde & M. Nadeu. (2012). Lenition of /d/ in spontaneous Spanish and Catalan. Proceedings of InterSpeech. The Conference of the International Speech Communication Association (ISCA). Portland, Oregon. (4 p. )
: Simpson, R. (2004). Stylistic features of academic speech: The role of formulaic expressions. En U. Connor & T. Upton (Eds.), Discourse in the professions. Perspectives from corpus linguistics (pp. 37-64). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
: Singer, M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Children learn when their teacher's gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 2, 85-89.
: Siu, Albert. (2015). Screening for speech and language delay and disorders in children aged 5 years or younger: US Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation Statement. Pediatrics,136(2), 474-481. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-1711.
: Slegers, A., Filiou, R. P., Montembeault, M. & Brambati, S. M. (2018). Connected speech features from picture description in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, (Preprint), 1-26.
: Smirnova, A. (2009). Reported speech as an element of argumentative newspaper discourse. Discourse Communication 3(1), 79-103.
: Smit, A. B., L. Hand, J.J. Frelinger, J. E. Bernthal y A. Bird. (1990). The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 779-798.
: Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child development, 43(2), 549-565. [304]https://doi.org/10.2307/1127555
: Snow, Catherine E., A. J. L. Arlman-Rupp, Y. Hassing, J. Jobse, Jan Joosten y Jan Vorster. 1976. Mothers’ speech in three social classes, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5,1: 1-20.
: Soares, C. & Grosjean, F. (1984). Bilinguals in a monolingual and a bilingual speech mode: The effect on lexical access. Memory and Cognition 12(4), (pp. 380-386).
: Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27(4), 501-532. [306]https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002
: Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 35(4), 869. [308]https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763
: Solé, Maria-Josep. 1992. Phonetic and phonological processes: the case of nasalization, Language and speech, 35 (1-2): 29-43.
: Speech Style Casual [94]Ávila-Jiménez, 1996 (Puerto Rico)
: Stam, G. (2006). Thinking for speaking about motion: L1 and L2 speech and gesture. IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 44(2), 145-171. doi:10.1515/IRAL.2006.006
: Steriade, Donca. 2001. Directional Asymmetries in Place Assimilation: A Perceptual Account, em E. Hume e K. Johnson (eds.), The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology , New York, Academic Press: 219-250.
: Stevenson, R. y Vitkovitch, M. (1986). The comprehension of anaphoric relations. Language and Speech, 29, 335-60.
: Stibbard, R. (2001). Vocal expression of emotions in non-laboratory speech (Tesis doctoral), University of Reading, Reading, Reino Unido.
: Stobbe, L. (2005). Doing machismo: Legitimating speech acts as a selection discourse. Gender work and Organization, 12(2), 105-123.
: Stothard, S., Snowling, M., Bishop, D., Chipchase, B. & Kaplan, C. (1998). Language impaired preschoolers: A follow up into adolescence. Journal of speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41, 407-418.
: Strange, V., & Shafer, V. L. (2008). Speech perception in second language learning: the re-education of selective perception. En Edwards J. G. , & M. L. Zampini (Eds.), Phonology and Second Language Acq uisition (pp. 153-192). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
: Strange, W. (2011). Automatic selective perception (ASP) of first and second language speech: A working model. Journal of phonetics, 39(4), 456-466.
: Street Jr, R. L. & Hopper, R. (1982). A Model of Speech Style Evaluation. In E. Bouchard Ryan & H. Giles (eds.), Attitudes Toward Language Variation: Social and Applied Contexts (175-188). Edward Arnold.
: Strik, H., Truong, K., de Wet, F. & Cucchiarini, C. (2009). Comparing different approaches for automatic pronunciation error detection. Speech Communication, 51(10), 845-852.
: Stromswold, K. (2008). The genetics of speech and language impairments. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(22), 2381-2383.
: Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1979). Speech Production. Trabajo presentado en el Ninth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. 6- 11 de agosto, Copenhague – Dinamarca. University of Copenhagen.
: Sun, L. y Nippold, M. (2012). Narrative writing in children and adolescents: Examining the literate lexicon. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 43(1), 2-13. [148]https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0099)
: SungHee, K., Frisina, R. D., y Frisina, D. R. (2006). Effects of age on speech understanding in normal hearing listeners: Relationship between the auditory efferent system and speech intelligibility in noise. Speech Communication, 48(7), 855-862. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2006.03.004
: Sutherland, Dean y Guillon, Gail. 2005. "Assessment of Phonological Representations in Children with Speech Impairment", en Language en Speech & Hearing Services in Schools 36 (4), pp. 294-307.
: Swales, J. (2004). Evaluation in academic speech: First forays. Academic discourse: new insights into evaluation. Bern: Peter Lang, 31-53.
: Swerts, Marc, Eva Strangert e Mattias Heldner. 1996. F[0] declination in read-aloud and spontaneous speech, Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Spoken Language (ICSLP 96), Philadelphia: 1501-1504.
: T'Hart, J.; Collier, R. & Cohen, A. 1990. A perceptual study of intonation. An experimental-phonetic approach to speech melody. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
: Taavitsainen, I. & A. Jucker (2008). Speech acts now and then. Towards a pragmatic history of English. En A. H. Jucker & I. Taavitsainen (Eds.), Speech Acts in the History of English (pp. 1-26). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
: Tabensky, A. (2001b). Gesture and speech rephrasings in conversation. Gesture, 1(2), 213-235.
: Tanenhaus, M. K. & Trueswell, J. C. (1995). Sentence comprehension. En P. Eimas & J. Miller (Eds.), Handbook in Perception and Cognition, Volume 11: Speech, Language, and Communication (pp. 217-262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
: Tannen, D. (1986), “Introducing constructed dialogue in Greek and American conversational and literary narrative”, Direct and Indirect Speech, 31, pp. 311-332.
: Tapietes intermarry today with Wichís-Weenhayek, Chorotes, Tobas, Guaraníes, and Spanish speakers. Daily verbal interactions can develop in two or more languages within the same speech event (^[95]Ciccone, 2015).
: Thal, Donna J., Laureen O’hanlon, Mary Clemmons y LaShon Frailin 1999 "Validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax for preschool children with language impairment". Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 42, 2, 482-496. [100]https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4202.482.
: Thal, Donna, Donna Jackson-Maldonado y Dora Acosta 2000 "Validity of parent-report measure of vocabulary and grammar for Spanish-speaking toddlers". Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 43, 5, 1087-1100. [98]https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4305.1087.
: The basic elements of an intelligent tutor model include tutor, leaner, domain, speech processing, and error detection (^[41]Swartz & Yazdani, 2012). These components perform activities which together comprise the L2 teaching-learning process.
: The deictic 3^rd person pronominal reference is characterized by its exclusive character, i.e., it does not explicitly include possible audience of Bachelet’s speech, and it is directed to, or speak about, ‘third parties’ or ‘others’ (Nikitina, 2012).
: Thiessen, E.D., Hill, E. A. & Saffran, J.R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7 (1), 53-71.
: Thomas R. (2007). The Value of Speech Act Theory for Interpretation. En: Masters Seminary Journal pp.3-21. Sun Valley.
: Thomas, E. (2002). Sociophonetic applications of speech perception experiments. American Speech, 77(2), 115-138.
: Thompson, C., Shapiro, L., Ballord, K., Jacobs, B., Schneider, S. & Tait, M. (1997). Training and Generalized Production of Wh- and NP- Movement Structures in Agrammatic Aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 228-244.
: Thordardottir, E. (2008). Language specific effects of task demands on the manifestation of specific language impairment: A comparison of English and Icelandic. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 51, 922-937. Doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/068).
: Thornburg, L. & Panther, K. (1997). Speech act metonymies. En W. Liebert, G. Redeker & L. Waugh (Eds.), Discourse and perspectives in cognitive linguistics (pp. 205-219). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.
: Tobin, Y. (2002). Phonology as human behavior: Theoretical implications and cognitive and clinical applications. En E. Fava, (Ed.), Clinical Linguistics: Theory and applications in speech pathology and therapy (pp. 3-22). Amsterdam/Filadelfia: John Benjamin.
: Tomasello, M., Farrar, J. & Dines, J. (1983). Children speech revision for a familiar and unfamiliar adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 359-363.
: Tomblin, Bruce, Records, Nancy, Buckwalter, Paula, Zhang, Xuyang, Smith, Elaine y O'Brien, Marlea. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of speech language and hearing research, 40 (6), 1245-1260.
: Tomblin, Bruce; Hardy, James y Hein, Herman. 1991. "Predicting poor-communication status in preschool children using risk factors present at birth", en Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34 (5), pp. 1096-1105.
: Tomblin, Bruce; Records, Nancy; Buckwalter, Paula; Zhang, Xuyang; Smith, Elaine y O'Brien, Marlea. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Speech Language Hear. 40 (6), 1245-1260.
: Topic shift markers in parliamentary discourse: Analysis of President Zapatero’s speech [2011]
: Toro, J., Trobalon, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2005). Effects of backward speech and speaker variability in language discrimination by rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31 (1), 95-100.
: Toth, L., Hoffmann, I., Gosztolya, G., Vincze, V., Szatloczki, G., Banreti, Z. & Kálmán, J. (2018). A speech recognition-based solution for the automatic detection of mild cognitive impairment from spontaneous speech. Current Alzheimer Research, 15(2), 130-138.
: Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. En M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before the speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321-347). Cambrigde: Cambrigde University Press.
: Trim, J. (1963). Linguistics and speech pathology. En S. Mason (Ed.), Signs, signals and symbols: A presentation of the British approach to speech pathology and therapy (pp. 33-43). Springfield: Charles C. Thomas.
: Trofimovich, P. & Baker, W. (2006). Learning second language suprasegmentals: Effect of L2 experience on prosody and fluency characteristics of L2 speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(1), 1-30.
: Turell, M. T. (2010). The use of textual, grammatical and sociolinguistic evidence in forensic text comparison. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 17(2), 211-250.
: Turkstra, L., Ciccia, A. & Seaton, C. (2003). Interactive behaviors in adolescent conversation dyads. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 34(2), 117-142.
: Uccelli, P. & Paez, M.M. (2007). "Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills". Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 38, 225-236.
: Ukrainetz, T. & Gillam, R. (2009). The expressive elaboration of imaginative Narratives by children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 883-898.
: Urban, G. (1984). Speech about speech in speech about action. Journal of American Folklore 97, 310-328.
: Van Lancker Sidtis, D., Choi, J., Alken, A. & Sidtis, J. J. (2015). Formulaic language in Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease: Complementary effects of subcortical and cortical dysfunction. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 58(5), 1493-1506.
: Van der Houwen, F. (1998). Organizing discourse. Direct and Indirect Speech in Mexican Spanish. Linguistics in the Netherlands, 15(1), 123-134.
: Vanderslice, R. & Pearson, L. S. (1967). 'Prosodic features of Hawaiian English'. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 53, 156-66.
: Veenendaal, Nathalie J.; Groen, Margriet A., y Verhoeven, L. (2014). The role of speech prosody and text Reading prosody in children's reading comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 521-536
: Velázquez, E. P. (2016). Stability of Nahualt and Spanish Intonation Systems of Bilingual Nahualt Speakers from the Mexican Veracruz Huasteca Region. En Speech Prosody (pp. 1148-1152). DOI:10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-236.
: Veneziano, E., Sinclair, H., y Berthoud, I. (1990). From one word to two words: repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language, 17(3), 633-650. Doi: 10.1017/s0305000900010928.
: Veneziano, Eddy. 2013. A Cognitive-Pragmatic Model for the change from single-word to multiword speech: A constructivist approach. Journal of Pragmatics , 56: 133-50.
: Veneziano, Edy y Christophe Parisse. 2010. The acquisition of early verbs in French: Assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed speech, First Language , 30,3-4: 287-311.
: Venkatagiri, H., & Levis, J. (2007). Phonological awareness and speech comprehensibility: An exploratory study. Language Awareness, 16(4), 263-277. [128]https://doi.org/10.2167/la417.0
: Vigliocco, G; Butterworth, B; & Semenza, C. (1995) Constructing Subject-Verb Agreement in Speech: The Role of Semantic and Morphological Factors. In Journal of Memory and Language. (34) pp. 186-215.
: Vincent, D. & Perrin, L. (1999). On the narrative vs non-narrative functions of reported speech: A socio-pragmatic study. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 3(3), 291-313.
: Vitaljevna, A. (2009). Reported speech as an element of argumentive newspaper discourse. Discourse & Communication, 3(1), 79-103.
: Vitevitch, M. S. & Sommers, M. S. 2003. "The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighbordhood frecuency in speech production in younger and older adults". En Memory & Cognition, 31, pp. 491-504.
: Vitevitch, Michael S.; Paul A. Luce, Jan Charles-Luce and David Kemmerer. 1997. "Phonotactics and syllable stress: Implications for the processing of spoken nonsense words", Language and Speech, 40: 47-62.
: Vygotsky, L. (1961). Thought and speech. En S. Saporta (Ed.), Psycholinguistics: A book of readings (pp. 197-260). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
: Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and Speech. In R.W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky , Vol. 1, Problems of general psychology (pp. 39-285). New York: Plenum Press.
: WAUGH, L. R. (1995). "Reported Speech in Journalistic Discourse: The Relation of Function and Text" , en Text, vol. 15(1): 129-173.
: Waara, R. 2004. Construal, Convention, and Constructions in L2 Speech. En Achard, Michel y Niemeier, Susan (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics, Second Language Acquisition, and Foreign Language Teaching, Berlin, Alemania: Mouton de Gruyter, 51-76.
: Waksler, R. (2001). A new all in conversation. American Speech, 76(2), 128-138. [127]https://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-76-2-128
: Wallace, Ina. F., Berkman, Nancy D., Watson, Linda R., Coyne-Beasley, Tamera, Wood, Charles, Cullen, Katherine y Lohr, Kathleen N. (2015). Screening for speech and language delay in children 5 years old and younger: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 136(2), 448-462.
: Walsh, M. (1999). Interpreting for the transcript: Problems in recording Aboriginal land claim proceedings in northern Australia.The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law , 6(1), 161-195.
: Wei, J. (2013a). Corpus-based research on the development of theme choices in Chinese learners' English speech. Journal of Education and Practice, 4(16), 38-45.
: Weingarten, R., Nottbusch, G. & Will, U. (2004). Morphemes, syllables, and graphemes in written word production. En T. Pechmann & C. Habel (Eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to speech production (pp. 529-572). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
: Weissheimer, J. & Mota, M. 2009. "Individual differences in working memory capacity and the development of L2 speech production", in Issues in Applied Linguistics 17, pp. 93-112.
: Weissheimer, J. 2007. Working memory capacity and the development of L2 speech production. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Florianópolis, Brazil: Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina.
: Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant behavior and development, 7(1), 49-63.
: Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H., Humphrey, K. & Tees, R. C. (1981). Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perceptions. Child Development, 52, 349-355.
: Werker, J.F., Pons, F. , Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L. & Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103, 147-162.
: Werker, Janet y Tees, Richard. 1999. "Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis", en Annual Review of Psychology 50, pp. 509-535.
: Whitfield, J. A., Reif, A., & Goberman, A. M. (2018). Voicing contrast of stop consonant production in the speech of individuals with Parkinson disease ON and OFF dopaminergic medication. Clinical linguistics & phonetics , 32(7), 587-594.
: Wierzbicka, A. (1987). English speech act verbs. A semantic dictionnary. Sydney: Academic Press.
: Wilkinson, L., & Janks, H. (1998). Teaching Direct and Reported Speech from a Critical Language Awareness (CLA) Perspective. Educational Review, 50(2): 181-190.
: Willis, C., Capilouto, G. & Wright, H. (2012). Attention and off-topic speech in the recount of middle-age and enderly adults: A pilot investigation. Contemporany issues in Communication Sciences & Disorders, 39, 105-112.
: Wingfield, A. & Stine-Morrow, E. (2000). Language and speech. En F.I.M. Craik & T.A. Salthouse (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 359-416). Hillside: N.J. Erlbaum.
: Winsler, Adam, Fernyhough, Charles y Montero, Ignacio (eds.). (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development verbal self-regulation. Cambridge: C.U.P.
: Wode, Henning. (1996). Speech perception and L2 phonological acquisition. En P. Jordaens y J. Lalleman (eds.), Investigating Second Language Acquisition (pp. 321-353). Berlin, Alemania: Mouton de Gruyter.
: Wolfram, W. (1969). A Sociolinguistic Description of Detroit Negro Speech. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.
: Woll, B., & Sieratzki, J. (1998). Echo phonology: Signs of a link between gesture and speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(4), 531-532.
: Wolter, J. A., Wood, A. & D’Zatko, K. (2009). The influence of morphological awareness on first-grade children’s literacy development. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 40(3), 1-13.
: Wood, D. (2010). Formulaic Language and Second Language Speech Fluency: Background, Evidence and Classroom Applications. Continuum International Publishing.
: Woodward, J. Z. & Aslin, R.N. (1990). Segmentation cues in maternal speech to infants. Paper presented at 7th biennial meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, Montréal, Quebec, Canada.
: Wortham, S. (2010). Listening for Identity Beyond the Speech Event. Teachers College Record, 112, 2848-2871. Recuperado de [287]http://www.tcrecord.org/content.asp?contentid=15796.
: Wright, Richard. 2001. Perceptual cues in contrast maintenance, em Elizabeth Hume e Keith Johnson (eds.), The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology , San Diego, Academic Press: 251-277.
: Wunderlich, D. (1980). Methodological remarks in Speech Act Theory. In J. Searle, F. Kiefer, & M. Bierwish (Eds.). Speech Act Theory and Pragmatics (pp. 290-312). Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reidel Publishing Company.
: Ye, Z., Stolk, A., Toni, I. & Hagoort, P. (2017). Oxytocin modulates semantic integration in speech.
: Yu, D. & Deng, L. (2012). Automatic speech recognition. Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer.
: Yu, M.-C. ( 2004). Interlinguistic variation and similarity in second language speech act behavior. The Modern Language Journal, 88(1), 102-119.
: Zahn, C. & Hopper, R. (1985). Measuring language attitudes: the speech evaluation instrument. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 4. 113-123.
: Zamorano, J. (2004). Text generator, error analysis, and feedback. En R. Delmonte, P. Delcloque & S. Tonelli (Eds.), NLP and speech technologies in advanced language learning systems (pp. 87-91). Padova: Unipress.
: Zamuner, T., Gerken, L., & Hammond, M. (2004). Phonotactic probabili-ties in young children’s speech production. Journal of Child Language , 3(31), 515 - 536.
: Zangl, R. & Mills, D.L. (2007). Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6- and 13-month-old infants. Infancy, 11, 31-62.
: Zinn, C., Moore, J. & Core, M. (2005). Intelligent information presentation for tutoring systems. En O. Stock & M. Zancanaro (Eds.), Multimodal intelligent information presentation. Serie: Text, Speech and Language Technology (pp. 227-254). Kluver Academic Publishers.
: [100]Otheguy[101], Ricardo, Ana Celia Zentella y David Livert. [102]2007. Language Contact in Spanish in New York: Toward the formation of a speech community, Language 83, 4: 770-802.
: [109]Butterworth, Brian. 1980. Evidence from pauses in speech, en B. Butterworth (ed.), Language production I: Speech and talk, London, Academic Press: 155-176.
: [127]Cameron, Richard. [128]1998. A variable syntax of speech, gesture, and sound effect: Direct quotations in Spanish. Language Variation and Change 10. 43-83.
: [133]Clements, George N. e Elizabeth V. Hume.[134] 1995. The internal organization of speech sounds. Em John Goldsmith (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory, Cambridge, MA/ Oxford: Basil Blackwell: 245-306.
: [136]Kent, Ray e Charles Read. 2002. The acoustic analysis of speech. New York, Cengage Learning.
: [148]Muysken[149], Pieter. 2000. Bilingual Speech: A typology of code-mixing, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
: [155]Kent[156], Ray. D. e Houri K. Vorperian. 2013. Speech impairment in Down syndrome: a review, Journal of Speech and Language Learning Research, 56, 1: 178-–210.
: [164]Ward, Gregory y Betty Birner. 1996. A crosslinguistic study of postposing in discourse, Language and Speech, 39: 113-142.
: [177]Lehrer, Adrienne. 1998. Scapes, holics and thons: the semantics of combining forms, American Speech, 73 (1): 3-28.
: [185]Clements, George Nick e Elisabeth Hume. 1995. The internal organization of speech sounds, em J. Goldsmith (ed.), The Handbook of phonological theory, Massachussets, Blackwell: 245-306.
: [70]Bryant, Gregory y Jean Fox Tree. 2005. Is there an ironic tone o voice?, Language and speech, 48 (3): 257-277.
: [72]Cheang, Henry y Marc Pell. 2008. The sound of sarcasm, Speech communication, 50: 366-381.
: [74]Clements, George. 1990. The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification, in J. Kingston e M. E. Beckman (Orgs.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the grammar and the physics of speech, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press: 283-333.
: [76]Clements, George e Elizabeth Hume. 1995. The internal organization in speech sounds, in J. Goldsmith (Org.), The handbook of phonological theory, Cambridge, Blackwell: 245-306.
: ^11CÓMO CITAR ESTE ARTÍCULO Zwisler, J., & Cuellar, C. (2020). Sociolinguistic proximity in animal-directed speech. Lenguaje, 48(2), 354-368. doi: 10.25100/lenguaje.v48i2.7484
: _. (1986). "Say 'thank you' -or something". American Speech 61, 76-88.
: _____(1988) Exploring the recognition of guest words in bilingual speech. Language and Cognitive Processes 3, 233-274.
: _______ . (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. (C. Emerson, Ed.). Austin: Univeristy of Texas Press.
: _________ (1984). Speech acts, speakers and hearers. References and referential strategies in spanish. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
: syntactic structure as it emerges in L2 speech, it still remains to be seen whether this capacity can be expanded (and if so how) and how it can be best measured (Juffs & Harrington, 2011).
: van Eemeren, F. & Grootendorst, R. (1984). Speech Acts in argumentative discussions. Dordrecht: Foris.
: van der Houwen, F. (1998), “Organizing discourse. Direct and indirect speech in Mexican Spanish”, Linguistics in the Netherlands, 15, pp. 123-134.
: Álvarez, A. (1988). Martin Luther King's "I Have a Dream": The Speech Event As Metaphor. Journal of Black Studies, 337-357.