Termout.org logo/LING


Update: February 24, 2023 The new version of Termout.org is now online, so this web site is now obsolete and will soon be dismantled.

Lista de candidatos sometidos a examen:
1) first-person (*)
(*) Términos presentes en el nuestro glosario de lingüística

1) Candidate: first-person


Is in goldstandard

1
paper CH_corpusSignostxt426 - : Starting with the deictic centre I and finishing with the distant they, this scale shows “the movement from the proximal to the distal” (Adentunji, 2006: 180) in the use of pronominal references in political context. In his study of speeches by Casper Weinberger, (former United States Defense Secretary), Urban (1988) focuses on the use of the first-person plural pronoun we by defining its six different uses:

2
paper CO_FormayFuncióntxt288 - : Kamsá has nominative-accusative morphosyntactic alignment. S and A are marked differently on the verb than O. In the following examples, S and A for first person singular are marked with së while O is marked with s̈o. Note that the first-person singular pronoun «ats̈» is the same in all examples: as S (in example 15a ), A (in example 15b), and O (in example 15c). Most languages of the Andes have nominative-accusative alignment, whereas many languages of the Amazon have ergative, or partially ergative alignment; thus, Kamsá patterns more with Andean languages with respect to alignment.

3
paper CO_Lenguajetxt179 - : Lower Bermejo River Wichí has the following verbal strategies to encode first-person non-singular subject (^[148]Nercesian, 2014): i . first-person inclusive prefix (3a), ii. first-person exclusive free pronoun plus a first-person singular verbal prefix (3b), and iii. first-person exclusive free pronoun plus verb without person inflection (3c), and iv. first-person singular prefix plus the verbal plural clitic =hen (on intransitive verbs) (4).

4
paper CO_Lenguajetxt179 - : In this paper we propose that the peculiar first-person non-singular subject verbal indexes in Eastern Toba, Western Toba and Tapiete - when compared with genetically related languages - can be viewed as instances of contact-induced grammatical replication from Matacoan model languages (Maká, Nivaclé, and Wichí). The language internal resources used to replicate the first-person non-singular subject verbal split are different in the three replica languages: i . the use of the collective suffix -pi within the verbal pronominal domain in Eastern Toba, ii. the addition of a new verbal pronominal paradigmatic unit, a prefix to encode first-person restricted group, in Western Toba, and iii. the adoption of a morphological structure of first-person non-singular verbal split in Tapiete that is closer to the Matacoan model languages than to Tupi-Guaraní languages. The structural effects of contact-induced grammaticalization on the first-person non-singular verbal indexes of the replica languages are

5
paper CO_Lenguajetxt181 - : First-person singular and third-person subject pronoun variation: The case of Mexican Spanish in the U .S. state of Georgia

Evaluando al candidato first-person:


1) verbal: 9 (*)
3) singular: 5 (*)
4) non-singular: 5
5) pronoun: 5 (*)
6) prefix: 4
8) toba: 4 (*)
10) pronominal: 3 (*)
11) plus: 3
12) marked: 3

first-person
Lengua: eng
Frec: 74
Docs: 19
Nombre propio: 1 / 74 = 1%
Coocurrencias con glosario: 5
Puntaje: 5.884 = (5 + (1+5.39231742277876) / (1+6.22881869049588)));
Candidato aceptado

Referencias bibliográficas encontradas sobre cada término

(Que existan referencias dedicadas a un término es también indicio de terminologicidad.)
first-person
: 1. the representative: “a generic first-person pronoun, usually realized as the plural ‘we’ or ‘us’, that writers use as a proxy for a larger group of people” (^[111]Tang & John, 1999: 27).
: 9. Curnow, Timothy Jowan. 2002. Types of interaction between evidentials and first-person subjects. Anthropological Linguistics 44, 2. 178-196.
: Aijón Oliva, M. A. (2013). On the Meanings and Functions of Grammatical Choice: The Spanish First-Person Plural in Written-Press Discourse. Pragmatics. Quarterly Publication of the International Pragmatics Association (IPrA), Vol. 23, pp. 573-603.
: Carciu, O. (2009). An intercultural study of first-person plural references in biomedical writing. Ibérica, 18, 71-92.
: Limerick, P.P. (2020). First-person plural subject pronoun expression in Mexican Spanish spoken in Georgia. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics.
: Marshall, J. (2004). Living Systemic Thinking: Exploring Quality in First-Person Action Research. Action Research. 2(3), 309-329. Retrieved from [55]http://jmarshall.org.uk/Papers/2004%20Marshall%20LivingSystemicThinking.pdf.
: Proto Tupi-guaraní and other Tupian languages, for example Sirionó, Yuki, Izoceño (18), Avá-Guaraní, and Chané (19) (^[189]Crevels & Muysken, 2005; ^[190]Dietrich, 2009-2010; ^[191]Dietrich, 1986) encode first-person inclusive and exclusive subject through verbal prefixes.
: Tapiete, a Tupi-Guaraní language, has a prefix to encode first person inclusive subject (17a), and a circumfix to encode first-person exclusive subject (17b), which is composed of a third person prefix (17c) plus a first-person exclusive suffix (^[184]González, 2005)^[185]^8.
: Williams, I. A. (2010). Cultural differences in Academic Discourse. Evidence from first-person verb use in the methods sections of medical research articles. Special Issue of International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(2), 214-239.